We would like to suggest that cortical spacing is the critical factor in determining the strengths of the contextual influences on perceived orientation. Previously, Motter and Simoni (
2007) suggested that the critical region for crowding may correspond to a constant cortical separation. Initial measurements of this critical region were done by Bouma (
1970), whose “rule of thumb” is now known as “Bouma's Law” (Pelli & Tillman,
2008). If we accept Bouma's Law for the critical region, then Motter and Simoni's suggestion can be written as
where
M(
w) is the cortical magnification factor (in millimeters) as a function of eccentricity
w (in degrees), and
c is a constant. If
Equation 3 is to hold for all
w > 0, then
M(
w) has to be logarithmic.
7
In order to be consistent with Bouma's Law, our model uses a logarithmic approximation to Duncan and Boynton's (
2003) formula for our non-foveal stimuli:
A graphical comparison between Duncan and Boynton's
M(
w) and our
M′(
w) is provided in
Figure 9.
In our model, bias (−
μ, defined empirically in
Equation 2) reflects the difference between an assimilative force and a repulsive force,
8 each of which varies in proportion to the difference between target and flank orientations Δ
θ, and a decaying function of the cortical distance between the target and its nearest flanker. In its most successful form, our model can be expressed as
where
D is the cortical distance and
M ass,
σ ass,
M rep, and
λ rep are potentially free parameters, but see below.
As described in
Equation 5, our model has the assimilative force decaying as a Gaussian function of cortical distance and the repulsive force decaying as an exponential function. This is illustrated in
Figure 10a. Preliminary simulations with an exponentially decaying assimilative force did not produce the rapid change, which can be seen in
Figure 10b, from assimilation with 5-deg flankers separated from the target by about 15% of its eccentricity to repulsion with 5-deg flankers separated from the target by about 40% of its eccentricity. That is because exponential functions change most rapidly at their peak (here at
D = 0). Gaussian functions change most rapidly away from their peak (i.e., when
D > 0).
For the present purposes, we apply
Equation 4 to the eccentricity of each target
w target and its most peripheral flanker
w flank to approximate these distances:
In the rightmost factor of the preceding equation, Δ
θ is the (unsigned) orientation difference between the target and its flanks, and
k is a small, positive constant. Inclusion of this factor was motivated by the existence of orientation columns: In small regions of visual cortex, the proximity of any two neurons tends to increase with the similarity of their orientation preferences (Hubel & Wiesel,
1974). From a functional standpoint, inclusion of this term effectively reduces the influence of more oblique flankers on orientation bias.
Some of the model parameters have natural constraints. For the decay, we can constrain all four of these parameters to the positive numbers: M ass, σ ass, M rep, and λ rep. The first of these parameters has a further constraint, stemming from the fact that assimilation can be no greater than 100% of the difference between target and flank orientations, i.e., M ass ≤ 1. One final constraint concerns the constant k. While it does not seem unreasonable to imagine any of the other four parameters changing with signal strength, it does not really make sense that the distance between orientation columns would also change. Therefore, when finding the best possible fit of the most general, yet sensible form of this model to our data, we allowed 9 parameters to vary freely: one value for k, plus 2 values for each of the other four parameters (one for noisy stimuli, the other for noise-free stimuli).
As noted above, our data suggest that the repulsive force must decay more slowly with cortical distance than the assimilative force. Also noted above is the evidence suggesting an increase in either the strength or the extent of the assimilative force or a decrease in either the strength or the extent of the repulsive force, when the signal-to-noise ratio decreases.
All of the data we collected are summarized by the open and solid symbols in
Figure 10. We obtained 40 predictions from our model; one for each condition in Experiments 1–3, not counting repeats. The r.m.s. standard error of these 40 predictions was 1.3° when the most general form of the model (i.e., the one with 9 free parameters) is the maximum-likelihood fit to all the data.
In an attempt at parsimony, we obtained fits to several nested models (i.e., with fixed parameter values). Chi-square tests (see
1) suggest that two of these were significantly inferior [1 −
χ (1) 2(−2 ln Λ) < 0.02] to the more general (9-parameter) model. In one of these inferior fits, we fixed
k = 0. In two others, we either forced the high-SNR and low-SNR values of
M ass to be the same or we forced the high-SNR and low-SNR values of
σ ass to be the same. On the other hand, two nested models were not significantly inferior. (Comparison of the generalized likelihood ratio with chi-square suggests 1 −
χ (1) 2(−2 ln Λ) > 0.25.) In one of these models, there was only one value for
M rep (i.e., the same for both signal-to-noise ratios). In the other, there was only one value for
λ rep. Either of these models would have been suitable for illustration in
Figure 10. We selected the latter, simply because it produced a slightly better fit. When both
M rep and
λ rep were forced to remain invariant with SNR, the fit was significantly inferior.
Many aspects of the data are faithfully reproduced by the model, including the non-monotonic effect of eccentricity on the tilt illusion for unscaled stimuli, the reduced effect of eccentricity on M-scaled stimuli, and the switch from assimilation to repulsion with increased separation between the target and the ±5° flankers.
Following Murakami and Shimojo (
1993), we have replotted all of our data in terms of the cortical distance between the target and its most eccentric flanker (see
Figure 11). This allows readers to form an immediate appraisal of our suggestion that the cortical distance is what determines the influence flankers will have on a target's apparent orientation. When expressed as a fraction of flanker tilt, all our data form curves shaped like half the cross-section of a Mexican hat. Our model (the black curves) perhaps does not quite capture the rapidity with which assimilation changes to repulsion (at a cortical distance of approximately 0.5 mm), but otherwise it seems to fit the data rather well.