Figure 4 shows the predictions of these three linear models for horizontal square-wave corrugations, for each of our 5 subjects individually as well as for the data averaged across subjects. In each case, the blue squares show the psychophysical data, and the black symbols show the predictions of the three models. We show the predictions both as disparity thresholds for the square-wave corrugations (upper panels) and as the ratio of the thresholds for the two corrugations. The goodness of fit between the predicted thresholds and the data is represented by
χ 2; the lower the values, the better the fit. Note that the predictions from the three models are identical for spatial frequencies higher that 0.4 cycle/deg, so both here and in the next figure, the thresholds measured at low spatial frequencies are critical for distinguishing between the models.
We consider first the mdh model (∇ symbols). Since a square wave's higher harmonics have much lower amplitude than its fundamental, and the disparity sensitivity function measured with horizontal sine corrugations is relatively shallow, the fundamental is in most cases the first component to rise above threshold. Thus, the mdh model usually predicts that the ratio of disparity threshold with horizontal sine waves to that with horizontal square waves is 4/π = 1.273, the amplitude ratio of their fundamental components. This model predicts experimental thresholds well in all subjects.
The rms model (
) produces very similar predictions to the mdh. The mean of the
χ rms 2 values over the five subjects is 6.92, slightly worse than the mean of 4.02 for
χ mdh 2. In almost all cases, the peak model (Δ) is far worse at predicting the experimental data.
We conclude that both the rms and mdh models are adequate to account for detection thresholds for square-wave horizontally oriented disparity corrugations. Thus, the data so far do not enable us to distinguish clearly between single-channel and multi-channel models for disparity detection.
Figure 5 shows the results with the predictions for vertical square-wave corrugations. The most noticeable difference in the data, compared to horizontal corrugations, is the high ratio of sine/square thresholds, well in excess of 4/
π at low frequencies. The mdh model (∇) does go some way toward capturing this. For vertically oriented corrugations, the threshold ratio predicted by the mdh model can exceed the classic value of 4/
π. This interesting effect is due to the very steep increase in threshold at low frequencies, which does not occur for horizontally oriented disparity corrugations, nor in the luminance domain. As frequency increases from 0.05 cycle/deg, disparity thresholds for sine corrugations fall faster than the 1/
n decay in harmonics of the square wave. This means that it is possible for the square-wave's fifth harmonic at 5
f 1, for example, to be above threshold, while its fundamental at
f 1 is still undetectable. Thus, the mdh model produces threshold ratios approaching 5 in some subjects. However, its threshold ratios are still substantially smaller than those observed experimentally: subjects do much better on the square-wave corrugations than predicted by this model.
The peak model (Δ), in contrast, predicts that subjects do too well on the square-wave corrugations; its predicted thresholds are consistently too low at the lowest spatial frequencies. However, the rms model (
) shows the best fit of all three models, capturing the data remarkably well for all subjects and frequencies, without any free parameters. Since this model also accounted well for the horizontal data (
Figure 4), as far as our data are concerned, a single linear channel could underlie the detection of both horizontally and vertically oriented square-wave corrugations.