It is well known that the optical aberrations of the eye impose a fundamental limit to visual resolution (Campbell & Green,
1965). The pattern of ocular aberrations is unique for each individual (Porter, Guirao, Cox, & Williams,
2001), and it has been hypothesized that the visual system may have evolved a neural mechanism to optimize perception by minimizing the effects of these persistent aberrations (Artal et al.,
2004). Evidence suggests that the visual benefit of correcting high-order aberrations might not be fully realized due to neural mechanisms that naturally compensate for the aberrations of the eye (Artal et al.,
2004; Chen, Artal, Gutierrez, & Williams,
2007; Sabesan & Yoon,
2010). This is supported by the finding that the best subjective image quality is experienced when an observer views a stimulus through an aberration profile that is only partially corrected (Chen et al.,
2007). This has led to the suggestion that some of the visual benefits of the correction of ocular aberrations may not be realized immediately or may be undone by the nervous system's compensation for the previous aberration state of the eye (Artal et al.,
2004; Villegas, Alcón, & Artal,
2008). AO correction of the aberrations of the eye has been shown to improve optical quality both for imaging the retina and for delivering high-resolution stimuli (Liang, Williams, & Miller,
1997). Combined real-time imaging and stimulus delivery in the AOSLO has the potential to allow the performance of the visual system to be assessed unobstructed by the limitations imposed by the imperfect optics of the eye. However, the question remains as to whether the adult human visual system can take full advantage of the unprecedented image quality that is afforded by AO immediately, or if there is some learning period during which a person needs to adapt to the new pattern of aberrations present while viewing an AO-corrected image to obtain the best visual resolution possible. It has previously been shown that a visual benefit can be realized immediately after correction of the eyes' high-order aberrations, improving visual performance in both contrast sensitivity and visual acuity tasks (Liang et al.,
1997; Rossi & Roorda,
2010; Rossi, Weiser, Tarrant, & Roorda,
2007; Williams et al.,
2000; Yoon, Jeong, Cox, & Williams,
2004; Yoon & Williams,
2002). However, no study to date has examined whether AO-corrected performance on a visual resolution task improves with training, or if performance is different when correcting aberrations over a large or a small pupil.