In color constancy experiments, observers are commonly asked to match the appearance of two targets embedded in different illumination contexts (asymmetric matching), or to match one target to an internal reference of gray (achromatic settings). It is not clear whether these kinds of tasks are optimal for measuring constancy; in natural viewing conditions, illuminant changes often cause the color appearance of a surface to change, for instance across a shadow boundary, without affecting our judgment of the reflectance of the surface (e.g., Reeves, Amano, & Foster,
2008; Zaidi & Bostic,
2008). In other words, identical color appearance is not necessary for correct identification. Some more recent studies have investigated color constancy with tasks that do not require observers to match the appearance of stimuli across contexts (e.g., Bramwell & Hurlbert,
1996; Craven & Foster,
1992; Foster, Amano, & Nascimento,
2006; Zaidi & Bostic,
2008). Foster et al. (
2006) adopted an operational approach to study color constancy in natural images. They found observers to be able to reliably discriminate between changes in the illuminant and changes in the reflectance of a test surface embedded in the scene; color constancy indices calculated from discrimination performance varied between 0.69 and 0.97. Zaidi and Bostic (
2008) studied color constancy in real scenes with a forced-choice paradigm, where observers had to indicate which of four objects, placed in two contexts with different illuminants, was different from the other three. Zaidi and Bostic found that observers were often good at making this judgment, but that they made some systematic errors that could be described with a suboptimal similarity-based strategy. For the present study, we chose color classification as an alternative for matching, following a number of studies that have used it successfully to measure chromatic adaptation and color constancy (e.g., Amano & Foster,
2008; Chichilnisky & Wandell,
1999; Hansen, Walter, & Gegenfurtner,
2007; Olkkonen, Hansen, & Gegenfurtner,
2009; Smithson & Zaidi,
2004; Speigle & Brainard,
1996; Troost & de Weert,
1991; Uchikawa, Uchikawa, & Boynton,
1989).