For highly incoherent illumination, i.e., ideally where light at each point of the pupil is incoherent from that of neighboring points, the light would not interfere in the retinal plane and the predicted outcome with annular and half-annular apertures becomes identical to that of the standard Maxwellian SCE-I configuration. If so, it would be correct to integrate
Equation 1 across the pupil to determine its impact on vision. The results shown in
Figure 4 are in fair agreement with this view. However, the annular, and in particular, the half-annular apertures reveal a larger directionality factor than for the traditional Maxwellian configuration as apparent in
Table 1. For incoherent light, any displacement of the annular or half-annular pupil image from its location of symmetry at the peak of the SCE-I visibility function would lead to an uneven weighting of the integrated SCE-I function. In such a case, the part of the illumination nearest to the peak would impact more on the distribution and thus a higher directionality factor could be obtained. A numerical integration of
Equation 1 suggests a 22% increase of the directionality factor for a 0.5-mm displacement of a full annular aperture from the SCE-I peak location (estimated on a 2-mm pupil) and a 48% increase for a half-annular aperture displaced accordingly (estimated on a 6-mm pupil). This may well suffice to explain the deviations observed for the case of incoherent light. However, also the fact that white light was used for the traditional Maxwellian illumination study may explain the slightly different directionality factor observed (Stiles & Crawford,
1933).
The coherent case implies an absence of the SCE-I for any annular pupil (and thus also for the entire pupil) whereas the incoherent case implies that the SCE-I function can be added along the ring illumination and thus integrated across the entire pupil.