Our results demonstrate that our visual–motor system can rapidly and automatically adapt in response to conflicting information about self-motion, even with minimal visual input. Adaptation to visual displacement was detectable within 10 trials of exposure for either ground-flow or target-motion conditions, and this occurred even though subjects were unaware of the displacement and performed a secondary distracter task. The adaptation over the course of 20 trials corresponded to 20–25% of the visual displacement and produced a corresponding negative aftereffect when the cue conflict was subsequently removed.
We compared performance in two simulated environments, with and without global optic flow, to test whether visual feedback from the target is sufficient for locomotor adaptation. The ground-flow condition, which provided optic flow throughout the display, was more effective at inducing adaptation than the condition with a homogeneous ground plane, which provided only target motion. This was evidenced by a faster rate of initial adaptation and the larger aftereffect. Global optic flow is known to be a strong visual cue to self-motion, so its absence in the target-motion condition could explain the difference in adaptation. On the other hand, the target-motion condition also produced significant adaptation, despite the fact that the only visual motion information was from the target. Moreover, the reduction in heading error from the initial to final adaptation trials—a measure of total adaptation—was statistically indistinguishable for the two conditions. This null result may be due to lack of sensitivity, given that there was a detectable difference in magnitude of aftereffects. Nevertheless, it is surprising that the amount of adaptation was so similar for the two conditions, given the large difference in the amount of optic flow available.
In our experiment, walking performance during initial adaptation trials was similar for the ground-flow and target-motion conditions, as intended, so this factor was not confounded with the presence of global optic flow. In both conditions, observers aligned their physical direction of motion with the target when the conflicting visual information was first introduced, resulting in curved paths. During all adaptation stages, observers walked on paths that maintained an approximately constant visual heading error. The visual heading error reduced from initial to final adaptation trials, but there was no indication of a change of strategy, and no qualitative difference in performance for the ground-flow and target-motion conditions.
Although the target-motion condition provided minimal optic flow, it was sufficient for a subjective sense of moving toward a location in 3D space. Perception of self-motion might be the key factor for adaptation rather than how self-motion is specified by sensory information. This would be consistent with some evidence from adaptation of self-motion speed. Durgin, Fox, and Kim (
2003) and Durgin et al. (
2005) observed locomotor speed recalibration when blindfolded observers felt themselves to be stationary while walking or hopping on a treadmill (perhaps as a result of haptic contact with the handrails of the treadmill); this suggests that perceived speed of self-motion, rather than optic flow per se, was the controlling variable. Adaptation of self-motion direction might similarly depend primarily on perception of self-motion direction through space rather than on the presence and amount of optic flow. By this account, adaptation would be observed whenever sensory feedback provides a strong percept of self-motion that is in conflict with expectations, which could occur even without full-field optic flow.
Our results are generally consistent with those of Bruggeman et al. (
2007). As in their study, we observed adaptation in both minimal and full-field optic flow conditions and found that adaptation and aftereffects were larger with full-field optic flow. There was also similarity in the time course of adaptation. In the rich optic flow condition tested by Bruggeman et al., adaptation was detectable within the first 3 trials and reached an asymptote within 10 trials. This is comparable to the time course of adaptation for our ground-flow condition (
Figure 5). Adaptation was slower in our target-motion condition, which is also qualitatively consistent with the observations of Bruggeman et al.
We observed less difference between minimal and full-field optic flow conditions than Bruggeman et al. (
2007) in terms of both magnitude and speed of adaptation. The differences in simulated environments could account for this discrepancy. In our target-motion condition, the target was a circle on the ground plane rather than an infinitely tall pole in space and, therefore, provided better information about egocentric target location and self-motion relative to the target. On the other hand, our ground-flow condition was comparatively minimal relative to the rich flow condition tested by Bruggeman et al. Thus, the minimal and rich flow conditions tested by Bruggeman et al. differed in a more extreme way, which could account for the larger differences in performance.
When global optic flow is available, subjects could potentially make online steering adjustments to reduce visual heading error, which would produce straighter paths even prior to adaptation. This was observed by Bruggeman et al. (
2007) in their rich flow condition: heading error reduced over time during the first adaptation trial. In contrast, we found no evidence for direct use of optic flow to control steering. Visual heading error was approximately constant over the course of movement in the ground-flow condition, and adaptation across trials took the form of a shift in average visual heading error. The discrepancy may also be due to the different simulated environments used in our study and in Bruggeman et al. Warren et al. (
2001) tested both types of environments and observed larger visual heading errors for the ground-flow condition. However, Warren et al. observed some reduction in heading bias over time in their ground-flow condition, while our data were entirely consistent with a strategy of walking in the visual direction of the target.
We used a cover story and secondary task to disguise the purpose of our experiment, which may have attenuated the amount of adaptation. Redding, Clark, and Wallace (
1985) tested the effect of a secondary cognitive task on prism adaptation from walking and found that adaptation was significantly reduced by a simultaneous mental arithmetic or mental imagery task. Based on these findings, one might expect reduced adaptation in our conditions relative to a situation with no secondary task. This makes the adaptation in the minimal flow condition all the more striking, while making it unlikely that our results can be attributed to experimental demand characteristics (see Durgin et al.,
2009).
There are multiple possible sites of adaptation that could produce our observed changes in performance. Prior to adaptation, in both visual environments, observers appeared to be using a strategy of aiming their physical direction of motion toward the visual direction of the target. Reduction in heading error following repeated exposure could, therefore, be produced either by a general remapping of visual direction (Morton & Bastian,
2004; Rushton & Salvucci,
2001) or by locomotor-specific recalibration (Bruggeman & Warren,
2010), and this remapping could be either to optic flow or to perceived direction of walking (Rushton et al.,
1998). The speed and magnitude of adaptation observed here argues against a general remapping. As pointed out by Bruggeman et al. (
2007), studies of prism adaptation have observed very limited adaptation of perceived straight ahead (<10%) over a time course of minutes (Held & Bossom,
1961; Morton & Bastian,
2004; Redding & Wallace,
1985). This interpretation would also be consistent with the results of Bruggeman and Warren (
2010), who found no transfer of adaptation to visually guided tasks that did not involve locomotion. Indeed, in adaptation of perceived self-motion speed, recalibration has been shown to be specific not only to locomotion (Rieser et al.,
1995) but to the manner of locomotion (Durgin et al.,
2005), and, for hopping, to the specific limb involved in that locomotion (Durgin et al.,
2003).
The similar adaptation observed in ground-flow and target-motion conditions further constrains the locus of locomotor recalibration. In principle, global optic flow could be used for direct control of steering (Warren et al.,
2001), which would have the advantage of bypassing coordinate transformations between the eye, head, and body. We believe that this is unlikely to be the site of the adaptation observed here. Such a model would not be directly sensitive to a conflict between physical and visual headings; rather, adaptation would have to be driven by visual heading error. Recalibration of a direct visual control strategy would, therefore, be expected to strongly depend on the presence of global optic flow. We found that visual heading errors remained large and approximately constant after adaptation and that there was little difference between conditions with minimal and global optic flows. These results are more compatible with recalibration of a model that uses an integrated estimate of self-motion direction from visual and non-visual information. Global optic flow would then be advantageous but not essential, thereby accounting for the small observed difference between ground-flow and target-motion conditions.
In conclusion, our results demonstrate that the visual–motor system can rapidly adapt to discrepancies between physical and visual headings and that this adaptation can occur even with relatively minimal optic flow. Full-field optic flow increased the speed and magnitude of adaptation, but relative motion of the target was also sufficient to produce rapid adaptation of similar magnitude.