Free
Research Article  |   December 2010
Color and material perception: Achievements and challenges
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision December 2010, Vol.10, 19. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/10.9.19
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Laurence T. Maloney, David H. Brainard; Color and material perception: Achievements and challenges. Journal of Vision 2010;10(9):19. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.9.19.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

There is a large literature characterizing human perception of the lightness and color of matte surfaces arranged in coplanar arrays. In the past ten years researchers have begun to examine perception of lightness and color using wider ranges of stimuli intended to better approximate the conditions of everyday viewing. One emerging line of research concerns perception of lightness and color in scenes that approximate the three-dimensional environment we live in, with objects that need not be matte or coplanar and with geometrically complex illumination. A second concerns the perception of material surface properties other than color and lightness, such as gloss or roughness. This special issue features papers that address the rich set of questions and approaches that have emerged from these new research directions. Here, we briefly describe the articles in the issue and their relation to previous work.

Introduction
The classic perceptual correlates of object surface properties are lightness and color. Although there are notable exceptions (e.g., Gilchrist, 1977; Hochberg & Beck, 1954; Kardos, 1934; Mach, 1886/1959; for review, see Gilchrist, 2006), the preponderance of what we know about perception of these attributes comes from the study of simplified stimuli, flat–matte surfaces placed on a single plane under diffuse illumination. After more than a century, researchers have developed an accurate if still incomplete outline of how the human visual system assigns lightness and color descriptors in such “flat–matte” scenes (for reviews, see Brainard, 2009; Shevell & Kingdom, 2008). 
Over the past decade, research has increasingly begun to focus on two rich generalizations of the classic “flat–matte” paradigm (for reviews, see Adelson, 2008; Maloney, Gerhard, Boyaci, & Doerschner, 2011). The first generalization concerns the perception of lightness and color in scenes that approximate the three-dimensional environment we live in, with objects that need not be matte or flat and geometrically complex illumination. The second generalization concerns perception of material surface properties other than lightness and color, such as gloss or roughness. Both lines of move us toward the goal of understanding object surface perception as it operates in natural viewing. At the same time, the research presents novel challenges as we try to formulate theories that account for how the perception of lightness, color, gloss, roughness, and other material properties interact with one another as well as with object shape, object pose, and illumination geometry. 
In 2004, we edited a special issue in the Journal of Vision entitled “Perception of color and material properties in complex scenes” (Brainard & Maloney, 2004). This special issue is its sequel. Here, we briefly describe the articles in this issue. We have organized the overview according to broad themes. We have also set the current work in the context of its antecedents, with particular emphasis on work published since 2004. Overall, the current special issue illustrates the rich set of questions and promising approaches that are driving current interest in this maturing area of inquiry. 
Characterizing, estimating, and discriminating the light field
One of the most important themes in color and material perception is the role of scene illumination. In flat–matte scenes, the placement and directionality of light sources was little emphasized (Boyaci, Doerschner, Snyder, & Maloney, 2006). In everyday scenes, the light field need not be simple (Adelson & Bergen, 1991; Gershun, 1939) and material properties such as gloss or roughness are easier to assess in scenes with geometrically rich light fields (Fleming, Dror, & Adelson, 2003). 
Earlier in the decade, several papers (Koenderink, Pont, van Doorn, Kappers, & Todd, 2007; Pont & Koenderink, 2007; Rutherford & Brainard, 2002) directly assessed how observers judge the light field and changes in the light field (see also Foster & Nascimento, 1994). In addition, Dror, Leung, Adelson, and Willsky (2001) and Dror, Willsky, and Adelson (2004) measured the statistical properties of lighting in natural scenes while Doerschner, Boyaci, and Maloney (2007) tested whether human matte surface color perception can compensate for the potential complexity of light fields in natural scenes. 
In the current issue, three papers continue this theme. Schofield, Rock, Sun, Jian, and Georgson (2010) examine how human observers discriminate between changes in scene lighting and scene contents. They propose a special role for mechanisms associated with second-order spatial vision in discriminating illumination changes from reflectance changes. Gerhard and Maloney (2010a) measure how observers discriminate between changes in scene illumination and reflectance in three-dimensional scenes, under stimulus conditions where discrimination based on local illumination and monocular cues is not possible. They demonstrate that detection of a lighting change leads to enhanced ability to detect a simultaneous change in surface reflectance. Gerhard and Maloney (2010b) propose a model of light change detection, based on earlier work by Pentland (1982), and show that this model accounts for their data in detail. 
Complex light fields and surface color/lightness perception
In parallel with direct assessment of the perception of the light field, the past decade has seen a slew of papers that study how the visual system achieves color and lightness constancy in the context of spatially complex light fields (e.g., Bloj & Hurlbert, 2002; Bloj, Kersten, & Hurlbert, 1999; Bloj et al., 2004; Boyaci, Doerschner, & Maloney, 2004; Boyaci et al., 2006; Boyaci, Maloney, & Hersh, 2003; Hedrich, Bloj, & Ruppertsberg, 2009; Kraft, Maloney, & Brainard, 2002; Ripamonti et al., 2004; Robilotto & Zaidi, 2004; Snyder, Doerschner, & Maloney, 2005; Todd, Norman, & Mingolla, 2004; Werner, 2006; Yang & Maloney, 2001; Yang & Shevell, 2002; Zaidi & Bostic, 2008). Important earlier work includes Gilchrist (1977, 1980), Hochberg and Beck (1954), Ikeda, Shinoda, and Mizokami (1998), and Pessoa, Mingolla, and Arend (1996). 
Along these lines in the current issue, Radonjić, Todorović, and Gilchrist (2010) examine surface lightness perception in three-dimensional scenes with directional lighting and show how grouping principles such as adjacency and surroundedness can help organize the empirical phenomena. Olkkonen, Witzel, Hansen, and Gegenfurtner (2010) study color categorization for real surfaces and daylight illuminants. An entire room with controlled illumination served as the laboratory. They find that color categorization was little changed by marked changes in daylight illumination. 
Surface material perception: Gloss, roughness
A very active area of research is the assessment of lighting and environmental conditions that affect the perception of material properties such as gloss and roughness, and how these properties interact with the perception of color and lightness. Important early work includes Beck and Prazdny (1981) and Nishida and Shinya (1998). Pellacini, Ferwerda, and Greenberg (2000) used scaling methods to determine the perceptual dimensions of gloss perception using stimuli rendered via computer graphics, as did Obein, Knoblauch, and Viénot (2004) using stimuli consisting of real illuminated objects. 
Fleming et al. (2003) measured asymmetric matching performance with stimuli rendered under different real-world illuminants. Berzhanskaya, Swaminathan, Beck, and Mingolla (2005) studied how the perception of gloss propagates from highlights across the surface of three-dimensional objects. 
Motoyoshi, Nishida, Sharan, and Adelson (2007; Sharan, Li, Motoyoshi, Nishida, & Adelson, 2008; for review, see Adelson, 2008) proposed a model of material perception based on statistical moments of the luminance pixel and sub-band histograms of images, most notably histogram skewness. Anderson and Kim (2009), however, questioned whether histogram skewness provided significant explanatory power in the absence of explicit consideration of surface geometry. 
Ho, Landy, and Maloney (2006; Ho, Maloney, & Landy, 2007) used forced-choice methods to examine how texture (roughness) perception is affected by scene illumination and observer viewpoint. Emrith, Chantler, Green, Maloney, and Clarke (2010) used scaling methods to investigate perception of roughness. 
Most recently, Doerschner, Boyaci, and Maloney (2010) tested whether human observers have self-consistent perception of gloss and show that they do when the percepts are assessed using forced-choice, but not asymmetric matching, methods. 
Surface material perception is represented by a number of papers in this issue; Kim and Anderson (2010) extend their work on the limits of the explanatory power of simple luminance histogram statistics. Wijntjes and Pont (2010) systematically investigate interactions between relief height (as reported by Ho, Landy, & Maloney, 2008) and histogram skewness (Motoyoshi et al., 2007) and develop a model of the physical factors that determine perceived glossiness. They reject the skewness hypothesis in favor of a novel hypothesis they develop. 
Sakano and Ando (2010) examine the effect of binocular disparity and head movement and find that both factors enhance perceived glossiness. Wendt, Faul, Ekroll, and Mausfeld (2010) likewise explore the factors that affect gloss and lightness constancy. They find that motion, disparity, and color all improve the constancy of gloss matches across changes of object shape, but that only motion and color improve the constancy of corresponding lightness matches. Doerschner et al. (2010) examine gloss perception in scenes with high-dynamic ranges of illumination and report a gloss illusion. 
Interactions
There are several studies that examined interactions among different material properties. Ho et al. (2008) used a conjoint measurement design and rendered stimuli to show that surface roughness affected observers' judgments of surface glossiness and vice versa. The degree of contamination was small. Xiao and Brainard (2008), using rendered stimuli, assessed how the presence of specular highlights affects the color appearance of three-dimensional objects and showed that the visual system stabilizes color appearance with respect to material variation. Yoonessi and Zaidi (2010) examined the role of color in recognizing material changes. 
In this issue, Giesel and Gegenfurtner (2010) systematically investigate color perception for real objects made of different materials varying in roughness and gloss from smooth and glossy to matte and corrugated. They show that hue is quite stable across their manipulations, but that other attributes interact. Olkkonen and Brainard (2010) study how changes in real-world illumination affect perceived glossiness and lightness with emphasis on testing independence principles. They show, for example, that the effect of geometric changes in the light field on perceived glossiness is independent of the diffuse reflectance component of the surfaces. 
Novel themes
A number of papers in the current issue introduce novel themes. Wolfe and Myers (2010) examine visual search performance when targets and distractors are characterized by surface material. They find that, although it may be easy to discriminate “fur” or “stone,” searching for a patch of fur among the stones is difficult and time-consuming. Visual search based on material differences is inefficient. Motoyoshi (2010) examines how the relationship between highlights and shading triggers perception of translucency and transparency. Marín-Franch and Foster (2010) develop mathematical and experimental methods to assess how many perceptually distinct surfaces are present in natural scenes. Ged, Obein, Silvestri, Le Rohellec, and Viénot (2010) measure how perceived gloss relates to physical properties of actual surfaces. 
Goddard, Solomon, and Colin (2010) study the adaptable neural mechanisms responsible for surface color constancy. Boyaci, Fang, Murray, and Kersten (2010) also consider mechanism. They report behavioral results showing how lightness across occlusion depends on spatially distant image features and show (using brain imaging) that human early visual cortex responds strongly to occlusion-dependent lightness variations. They conclude that early cortical processing of lightness is affected by three-dimensional scene interpretation. 
Another emerging theme, not represented in this issue, is the introduction of non-homogeneous reflectance into the objects whose color or other material properties are being judged (Ho et al., 2008; Hurlbert, Vurro, & Ling, 2008; Olkkonen, Hansen, & Gegenfurtner, 2008; Robilotto & Zaidi, 2006). Most real-world objects contain some degree of such non-homogeneity, and we expect this line to grow in importance over the next few years. 
Acknowledgments
This work is supported by NIH RO1 EY10016 (DHB) and the A. v. Humboldt Foundation (LTM). M. Olkkonen provided the JOV icon. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Laurence T. Maloney. 
Email: ltm1@nyu.edu. 
Address: 6 Washington Place, Room 877, New York, NY 10003, USA. 
Special issue articles
Boyaci H. Fang F. Murray S. O. Kersten D. (2010). Perceptual grouping-dependent lightness processing in human early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):4, 1–12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/4, doi:10.1167/10.9.4. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Doerschner K. Maloney L. T. Boyaci H. (2010). Perceived glossiness in high dynamic range scenes. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):11, 1–11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/11, doi:10.1167/10.9.11. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ged G. Obein G. Silvestri Z. Le Rohellec J. Viénot F. (2010). Recognizing real materials from their glossy appearance. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):18, 1–17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/18, doi:10.1167/10.9.18. [PubMed] [Article]
Gerhard H. E. Maloney L. T. (2010a). Detection of light transformations and concomitant changes in surface albedo. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):1, 1–14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/1, doi:10.1167/10.9.1. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Gerhard H. E. Maloney L. T. (2010b). Estimating changes in lighting direction in binocularly viewed three-dimensional scenes. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):14, 1–22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/14, doi:10.1167/10.9.14. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Giesel M. Gegenfurtner K. R. (2010). Color appearance of real objects varying in material, hue, and shape. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):10, 1–21, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/10, doi:10.1167/10.9.10. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Goddard E. Solomon S. Colin C. (2010). Adaptable mechanisms sensitive to surface color in human vision. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):17, 1–13, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/17, doi:10.1167/10.9.17. [PubMed] [Article]
Kim J. Anderson B. L. (2010). Image statistics and the perception of surface gloss and lightness. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):3, 1–17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/3, doi:10.1167/10.9.3. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Marín-Franch I. Foster D. H. (2010). Number of perceptually distinct surface colors in natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):9, 1–7, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/9, doi:10.1167/10.9.9. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Motoyoshi I. (2010). Highlight–shading relationship as a cue for the perception of translucent and transparent materials. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):6, 1–11, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/6, doi:10.1167/10.9.6. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Olkkonen M. Brainard D. H. (2010). Perceived glossiness and lightness under real-world illumination. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):5, 1–19, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/5, doi:10.1167/10.9.5. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Olkkonen M. Witzel C. Hansen T. Gegenfurtner K. (2010). Categorical color constancy for real surfaces. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):16, 1–22, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/16, doi:10.1167/10.9.16. [PubMed] [Article]
Radonjić A. Todorović D. Gilchrist A. (2010). Adjacency and surroundedness in the depth effect on lightness. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):12, 1–16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/12, doi:10.1167/10.9.12. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sakano Y. Ando H. (2010). Effects of head motion and stereo viewing on perceived glossiness. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):15, 1–14, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/15, doi:10.1167/10.9.15. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schofield A. J. Rock P. B. Sun P. Jian X. Georgeson M. A. (2010). What is second-order vision for Discriminating illumination versus material changes. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):2, 1–18, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/2, doi:10.1167/10.9.2. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wendt G. Faul F. Ekroll V. Mausfeld R. (2010). Disparity, motion, and color information improve gloss constancy performance. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):7, 1–17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/7, doi:10.1167/10.9.7. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wijntjes M. W. A. Pont S. C. (2010). Illusory gloss on Lambertian surfaces. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):13, 1–12, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/13, doi:10.1167/10.9.13. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wolfe J. M. Myers L. (2010). Fur in the midst of the waters: Visual search for material type is inefficient. Journal of Vision, 10, (9):8, 1–9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/9/8, doi:10.1167/10.9.8. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
References
Adelson E. A. (2008). Image statistics and surface perception Paper presented at Human Vision and Electronic Imaging XIII. Proceedings of the SPIE, 6806, 1–9.
Adelson E. H. Bergen J. R. (1991). The plenoptic function and the elements of early vision. In Landy M. S. Movshon J. A. (Eds.), Computational models of visual processing (pp. 3–20). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Anderson B. L. Kim J. (2009). Image statistics do not explain the perception of gloss and lightness. Journal of Vision, 9, (11):10, 1–17, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/9/11/10, doi:10.1167/9.11.10. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Beck J. Prazdny S. (1981). Highlights and the perception of glossiness. Perception & Psychophysics, 30, 407–410. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Berzhanskaya J. Swaminathan G. Beck J. Mingolla E. (2005). Remote effects of highlights on gloss perception. Perception, 34, 565–575. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bloj M. Hurlbert A. C. (2002). An empirical study of the traditional Mach card effect. Perception, 31, 233–246. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bloj M. Kersten D. Hurlbert A. C. (1999). Perception of three-dimensional shape influences colour perception through mutual illumination. Nature, 402, 877–879. [PubMed]
Bloj M. Ripamonti C. Mitha K. Greenwald S. Hauck R. Brainard D. H. (2004). An equivalent illuminant model for the effect of surface slant on perceived lightness. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):6, 735–746, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/6, doi:10.1167/4.9.6. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Boyaci H. Doerschner K. Maloney L. T. (2004). Perceived surface color in binocularly-viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):1, 664–679, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/1, doi:10.1167/4.9.1. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Boyaci H. Doerschner K. Snyder J. L. Maloney L. T. (2006). Surface color perception in three-dimensional scenes. Visual Neuroscience, 23, 311–321. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Boyaci H. Maloney L. T. Hersh S. (2003). The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in three-dimensional scenes. Journal of Vision, 3, (8):2, 541–553, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/3/8/2, doi:10.1167/3.8.2. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Brainard D. H. (2009). Bayesian approaches to color vision. In Gazzaniga M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (4th ed., pp. 395–408). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Brainard D. H. Maloney L. T. (2004). Perception of color and material properties in complex scenes. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):i, 1–3, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/i, doi:10.1167/4.9.i. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Doerschner K. Boyaci H. Maloney L. T. (2007). Testing limits on matte surface color perception in three-dimensional scenes with complex light fields. Vision Research, 47, 3409–3423. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Doerschner K. Boyaci H. Maloney L. T. (2010). Estimating the glossiness transfer function induced by illumination change and testing its transitivity. Journal of Vision, 10, (4):8, 1–9, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/4/8, doi:10.1167/10.4.8. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dror R. O. Leung T. K. Adelson E. H. Willsky A. S. (2001). Statistics of real-world illumination. Proceedings of the IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, 2, 164–171.
Dror R. O. Willsky A. S. Adelson E. H. (2004). Statistical characterization of real-world illumination. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):11, 821–837, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/11, doi:10.1167/4.9.11. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Emrith K. Chantler M. J. Green P. R. Maloney L. T. Clarke A. D. F. (2010). Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 27, 1232–1244. [CrossRef]
Fleming R. W. Dror R. O. Adelson E. H. (2003). Real-word illumination and the perception of surface reflectance properties. Journal of Vision, 3, (5):3, 347–368, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/3/5/3, doi:10.1167/3.5.3. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Foster D. H. Nascimento S. M. C. (1994). Relational colour constancy from invariant cone-excitation ratios. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 257, 115–121. [CrossRef]
Gershun A. (1939). The light field (P Moon & G Timoshenko, Trans. Journal of Mathematics & Physics, 18, 51–151.
Gilchrist A. L. (1977). Perceived lightness depends on spatial arrangement. Science, 195, 185–187. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gilchrist A. L. (1980). When does perceived lightness depend on perceived spatial arrangement? Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 527–538. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gilchrist A. L. (2006). Seeing black and white. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hedrich M. Bloj M. Ruppertsberg A. I. (2009). Color constancy improves for real 3D objects. Journal of Vision, 9, (4):16, 1–16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/9/4/16, doi:10.1167/9.4.16. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ho Y. X. Landy M. S. Maloney L. T. (2006). How direction of illumination affects visually perceived surface roughness. Journal of Vision, 6, (5):8, 634–648, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/6/5/8, doi:10.1167/6.5.8. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ho Y.-H. Landy M. S. Maloney L. T. (2008). Conjoint measurement of gloss and surface texture. Psychological Science, 19, 196–204. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ho Y.-X. Maloney L. T. Landy M. S. (2007). The effect of viewpoint on perceived visual roughness. Journal of Vision, 7, (1):1, 1–16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/7/1/1, doi:10.1167/7.1.1. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hochberg J. E. Beck J. (1954). Apparent spatial arrangements and perceived brightness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 263–266. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hurlbert A. Vurro M. Ling Y. (2008). Colour constancy of polychromatic surfaces [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 8, (6):1101, 1101a, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/8/6/1101, doi:10.1167/8.6.1101. [CrossRef]
Ikeda M. Shinoda H. Mizokami Y. (1998). Three dimensionality of the recognized visual space of illumination proved by hidden illumination. Optical Review, 5, 200–205. [CrossRef]
Kardos L. (1934). Ding und Schatten; Eine experimentelle Untersuchung über die Grundlagen des Farbsehens. In von J. A. Schumann F. Jaensch, E. R. Kroh O. (Eds.), Zeitschrift für psychologie and physiologie der sinnesorgane, ergänzungsband (p. 23). Leipzig, Germany: Verlag.
Koenderink J. J. Pont S. C. van Doorn A. J. Kappers A. M. L. Todd J. T. (2007). The visual light field. Perception, 36, 75–90. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kraft J. M. Maloney S. I. Brainard D. H. (2002). Surface-illuminant ambiguity and color constancy: Effects of scene complexity and depth cues. Perception, 31, 247–263. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mach E. (1886/1959). The analysis of sensation (Translated from the 5th German edition by S. Waterlow). New York: Dover.
Maloney L. T. Gerhard H. E. Boyaci H. Doerschner K. (2011). Surface color perception and light field estimation in 3D scenes. In Harris L. R. Jenkin M. R. M. (Eds.), Vision in 3D environments (pp. 65–88). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Motoyoshi I. Nishida S. Sharan L. Adelson E. H. (2007). Image statistics for surface reflectance perception. Nature, 447, 206–209. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nishida S. Shinya M. (1998). Use of image-based information in judgments of surface-reflectance properties. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 15, 2951–2965. [CrossRef]
Obein G. Knoblauch K. Viénot F. (2004). Difference scaling of gloss: Nonlinearity, binocularity, and constancy. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):4, 711–720, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/4, doi:10.1167/4.9.4. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Olkkonen M. Hansen T. Gegenfurtner K. R. (2008). Color appearance of familiar objects: Effects of object shape, texture, and illumination changes. Journal of Vision, 8, (5):13, 1–16, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/8/5/13, doi:10.1167/8.5.13. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pellacini F. J. A. Ferwerda J. A. Greenberg D. P. (2000). Toward a psychophysically-based light reflection model for image synthesis. Paper presented at the ACM SIGGRAPH, New Orleans, LA.
Pentland A. P. (1982). Finding the illuminant direction. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 72, 448–455. [CrossRef]
Pessoa L. Mingolla E. Arend L. E. (1996). The perception of lightness in 3-D curved objects. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 1293–1305. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pont S. C. Koenderink J. J. (2007). Matching illumination of solid objects. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 459–468. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ripamonti C. Bloj M. Hauck R. Kiran K. Greenwald S. Maloney S. I. (2004). Measurements of the effect of surface slant on perceived lightness. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):7, 747–763, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/7, doi:10.1167/4.9.7. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Robilotto R. Zaidi Q. (2004). Limits of lightness identification for real objects under natural viewing conditions. Journal of Vision, 4, (9):9, 779–797, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/9, doi:10.1167/4.9.9. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Robilotto R. Zaidi Q. (2006). Lightness identification of patterned three-dimensional real objects. Journal of Vision, 6, (1):3, 18–36, http://www.journalofvision.org/content/6/1/3, doi:10.1167/6.1.3. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Rutherford M. D. Brainard D. H. (2002). Lightness constancy: A direct test of the illumination-estimation hypothesis. Psychological Science, 13, 142–149. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sharan L. Li Y. Motoyoshi I. Nishida S. Adelson E. H. (2008). Image statistics for surface reflectance perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 25, 846–865. [CrossRef]
Shevell S. K. Kingdom F. A. A. (2008). Color in complex scenes. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 143–166. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Snyder J. L. Doerschner K. Maloney L. T. (2005). Illumination estimation in three-dimensional scenes with and without specular cues. Journal of Vision, 5, (10):8, [CrossRef]
Todd J. T. Norman J. F. Mingolla E. (2004). Lightness constancy in the presence of specular highlights. Psychological Science, 15, 33–39. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Werner A. (2006). The influence of depth segregation on colour constancy. Perception, 35, 1171–1184. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Xiao B. Brainard D. H. (2008). Surface gloss and color perception of 3D objects. Visual Neuroscience, 25, 371–385. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yang J. N. Maloney L. T. (2001). Illuminant cues in surface color perception: Tests of three candidate cues. Vision Research, 41, 2581–2600. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yang J. N. Shevell S. K. (2002). Stereo disparity improves color constancy. Vision Research, 42, 1979–1989. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yoonessi A. Zaidi Q. (2010). The role of color in recognizing material changes. Ophthalmology Physiology Optics, 30, 626–631. [CrossRef]
Zaidi Q. Bostic M. (2008). Color strategies for object identification. Vision Research, 48, 2673–2681. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×