Linear first-stage filtering with gain control. Linear spatial frequency channels were first proposed by Campbell and Robson (
1968) and are now accepted as the basis for early visual processing. More recent evidence suggests that while such mechanisms are approximately linear they have a non-linear transfer function, which is expansive for low-input values and compressive for larger inputs (Legge & Foley,
1980). This compression is now thought to be due a contrast gain control mechanism that pools input from many channels and across space (Foley,
1994) and has been proposed as an explanation for the compressive behavior of simple cells in primary visual cortex (Albrecht & Geisler,
1991; Heeger,
1992,
1993). However, the pooling process is far from uniform: masking (and indeed facilitation) depends on the relative, frequency, orientation, and spatial locations of the test and mask stimuli giving rise to complex patterns of behavior (Foley,
1994; Meese,
2004; Meese, Challinor, Summers, & Baker,
2009). Specifically, a given channel receives most masking from channels tuned to similar frequencies and orientations although the orientation tuning of masking is very broad (Foley,
1994). Thus we apply cross-channel gain control to our first-stage filters. Each filter has its own gain control pool with equal weight being given to all orientations in the pool but less weight given to frequencies distant from the preferred frequency of the filter in question. Because of the simple nature of our stimuli, we only modeled first-stage filters tuned to the image equivalent of 0.4 and 16 c/deg and ±45°. First-stage responses are given by
where
C i is the pre-gain control response of the
ith filter,
C a is the response of all filters with the same preferred frequency as the
ith filter,
C b is the response of filters with preferred frequency different to that of the
ith filter,
w is the weight applied to off-frequency filters in the gain pool,
p and
q represent exponents on the forward and gain control terms, respectively, and
s 1 is the semi-saturation constant. In line with other similar models, we set
p and
q to 2.0 (e.g., Meese et al.,
2009);
s 1 and
w were free parameters. Application of this gain control mechanism results in a first-stage transfer function that initially accelerates and then saturates (
Figure 7b) broadly consistent with both psychophysical “dipper” experiments (Legge & Foley,
1980) and physiology (Albrecht & Geisler,
1991; Ledgeway et al.,
2005).