Besides outward asymmetry, crowding has other spatial asymmetries. Crowding is (on the average) stronger in the upper visual field (He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator,
1996), stronger along the radial direction (Toet & Levi,
1992), and, other factors taken into account, somewhat stronger for items arranged horizontally (Feng, Jiang, & He,
2007). The most pronounced of these other asymmetries is the radial–tangential asymmetry. Crowding is 2–2.5 times stronger for items arranged along the radial (meridional) direction than along the tangential (isoeccentric) direction (Toet & Levi,
1992). Still, this is a weak effect compared to the outward asymmetry of crowding. The outward mask is, on the average, 4 times more disruptive than the inward mask in crowding (Petrov et al.,
2007). Besides, radial–tangential asymmetries are widespread in the peripheral vision and may merely reflect generic organization of early visual processing (e.g., V1 architecture). Examples of such generic asymmetry include grating resolution and visibility (Rovamo, Virsu, Laurinen, & Hyvarinen,
1982), curvature detection (Fahle,
1986), and motion detection (Scobey & van Kan,
1991). In our recent study, we demonstrated that, like crowding, surround suppression is significantly stronger in the radial direction (Petrov et al.,
2007). Surround suppression is the inhibition of contrast sensitivity by a surround mask, and in many aspects, surround suppression is similar to crowding but not in the asymmetry that is specific to crowding. Thus, the radial–tangential anisotropy is rather unspecific, which makes it a poor choice for probing links between crowding and attention. On the contrary, the outward asymmetry appears to be a hallmark property of crowding.