Past work on motion transparency has primarily focused on the conditions that lead to a transparency percept. For instance, previous studies have determined the minimal direction difference (Braddick, Wishart, & Curran,
2002; Mather & Moulden,
1980; Smith, Curran, & Braddick,
1999) or speed difference (Masson, Mestre, & Stone,
1999; Mestre, Masson, & Stone,
2001) necessary to perceive transparency (which are relatively large compared to non-transparent motion discriminations). Other studies have determined the maximum number of transparent surfaces that can be perceived simultaneously (Andersen,
1989; Edwards & Greenwood,
2005). Motion transparency stimuli are more complex than stimuli containing only a single motion direction and are processed with different efficiencies (Calabro & Vaina,
2006; Suzuki & Watanabe,
2009; Wallace & Mamassian,
2003). Several neural structures are involved in motion transparency, but the cortical area MT/V5 in primates seems critical (Muckli, Singer, Zanella, & Goebel,
2002; Qian & Andersen,
1994). Finally, several models have been proposed to account for the segregation and integration of local motion signals in a bottom-up process (Qian, Andersen, & Adelson,
1994; Snowden & Verstraten,
1999; Zanker,
2005), although higher level influences, such as attention, can affect the perception of transparent motion (Felisberti & Zanker,
2005; Lankheet & Verstraten,
1995; Valdes-Sosa, Cobo, & Pinilla,
2000). While there are numerous previous studies on these topics, the question of apparent depth ordering in motion transparency remains to be addressed.