Many species show the ability to perform path integration, including insects (Müller & Wehner,
1988,
1994; Wehner & Srinivasan,
1981), birds (Regolin, Vallortigara, & Zanforlin,
1995; von Saint Paul,
1982), and mammals (Etienne,
1992; Mittelstaedt & Mittelstaedt,
1982) including humans (Klatzky et al.,
1990; Loomis et al.,
1993). Etienne, Berlie, Georgakopoulos, and Maurer (
1998) summarized the literature to show that humans, dogs, hamsters, spiders, bees, and ants all showed similar return-to-origin behaviors after traveling along an L-shape outbound route. However, comparing to nonhuman animals, human participants showed limited path integration abilities (Klatzky et al.,
1990; Loomis et al.,
1993; Passini, Proulx, & Rainville,
1990). Klatzky, Beall, Loomis, Golledge, and Philbeck (
1999) argued that one possible reason might be the methodological difference between human and nonhuman path integration studies. Path integration in nonhuman species has been studied in both experimental tasks and natural foraging behaviors in which they are motivated to freely navigate through the environment. In contrast, path integration studies in humans have been focused on experimental tasks, primarily in
path completion tasks. In this paradigm, human participants travel along several pre-designed segments and then attempt to directly return to the starting point without the aid of direct perceptual cues of the paths. Human participants seldom had opportunity to freely navigate through the environment or voluntarily determine the structure of the paths. Thus, the effects of active exploration on human path integration remain unknown.