As can be seen in
Figures 1C and
1E, we propose that an endogenous saccade and/or reach plan directs attention. This could take the form of reach and saccade command signals sent to areas involved in perception that are modulated by attention such as V4 or MT (Motter,
1993; Reynolds, Pasternak, & Desimone,
2000; Spitzer, Desimone, & Moran,
1988; Treue & Martinez-Trujillo,
1999; Treue & Maunsell,
1996). It has been suggested that the attentional signal that modifies the activity of neurons in these areas originates from higher order areas in frontal or parietal cortex (Reynolds & Chelazzi,
2004). For example, the frontal eye field (FEF) has been shown to be involved in both eye movement planning and attention (Moore & Armstrong,
2003; Moore & Fallah,
2004; Schall & Boucher,
2007; Thompson, Biscoe, & Sato,
2005) and is connected to higher visual areas (Moore & Armstrong,
2003; Moore & Fallah,
2004; Neggers et al.,
2007). Alternatively, endogenous saccade command signals may be sent from FEF to the posterior parietal cortex (Khan et al.,
2009; Moore & Armstrong,
2003; Neggers et al.,
2007; Schall, Morel, King, & Bullier,
1995; Stanton, Bruce, & Goldberg,
1995), a key area involved in attention (Colby & Goldberg,
1999). Specifically, it has been proposed that the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) contains a saliency/relevance map that determines specific locations to be attended (Fecteau & Munoz,
2006; Goldberg, Bisley, Powell, & Gottlieb,
2006; Gottlieb,
2007; Steinmetz & Constantinidis,
1995). It may be that saccade command signals from FEF are used in LIP to determine where attention should be allocated. These signals are in turn sent back to higher visual areas involved in perception (Buffalo, Fries, Landman, Liang, & Desimone,
2010).