We observed a bilateral advantage for endogenous attentional shifts. However, there might be a few possible confounds in the testing procedure that might have given rise to these results. First, shifts across hemifields were symmetrical across the vertical midline, whereas shifts within a hemifield were not symmetrical across the horizontal midline. Enhanced neural activity has been observed at locations symmetrical to where a stimulus is presented (Hsieh & Tse,
2010). Further, there is evidence for neurons with bilaterally symmetrical receptive fields (Motter, Steinmetz, Duffy, & Mountcastle,
1987; Pigarev, Nothdurft, & Kastner,
2001; Steinmetz, Motter, Duffy & Mountcastle,
1987). These might have made it easier for stimuli in the opposite hemifield condition to be processed. Second, the final locations in the opposite hemifield condition (a, d, f, and i) were cued more often than those in the same hemifield conditions. Attention might have been preferentially allocated to these locations, despite our attempts to minimize this, leading to faster stimulus processing there. Finally, the shifts in the opposite hemifield condition were close to the vertical midline. The space immediately around the vertical midline is represented by bilateral or callosally connected neurons (e.g., Choudhury, Whitteridge, & Wilson,
1965; Hubel & Wiesel,
1967; Tootell, Mendola, Hadjikhani, Liu, & Dale,
1998), which might process both locations efficiently. Each or all of these might have induced faster shifts across hemifields than within a hemifield.