Next to flash lag, the subjective timing illusion that has attracted a great deal of attention in vision science is color–motion asynchrony. In a typical presentation, a green pattern moving upward and a red pattern moving downward are alternated at the rate of 1–2 Hz. Most observers find it difficult to tell which direction is associated with which color. In addition, when the direction change occurs about 60–100 ms earlier than the color change, the observers reliably bind the two attributes, confidently reporting that the events appear simultaneous (Moutoussis & Zeki,
1997). This effect is observed even when perceived color changes are dissociated from spectral changes (Zeki & Moutoussis,
1997). The magnitude of apparent asynchrony is affected by various factors including attention (Paul & Schyns,
2003) and stimulus saliency (Adams & Mamassian,
2004). It is significantly reduced when the direction change angle is changed from 180 to 90 deg (Arnold & Clifford,
2002; Bedell, Chung, Ogmen, & Patel,
2003). The apparent asynchrony is consistent with how the magnitude of a color-contingent MAE changes with the relative timing of color and motion (Arnold, Clifford, & Wenderoth,
2001). The apparent synchrony obtained with binding judgments for repetitive changes diminishes or disappears for temporal-order judgments between a single color change and a single direction change (Aymoz & Viviani,
2004; Bedell et al.,
2003; Nishida & Johnston,
2002; Viviani & Aymoz,
2001; see also Amano, Johnston, & Nishida,
2007; Linares & López-Moliner,
2006). One interpretation of the color–motion asynchrony is that it reflects asynchronous awareness of color and motion, i.e., perceptual latency is longer for motion than for color (Moutoussis & Zeki,
1997; Zeki,
2003; Zeki & Bartels,
1999). This is a brain time account. Alternatively, the illusion may be caused by an error in generating proper neural codes to represent subjective time. According to the time marker hypothesis (Nishida & Johnston,
2002), color–motion asynchrony results from matching inappropriate time markers (salient features), with a color change being matched with a position change (motion) rather than with a motion direction change. This is because color change is a first-order temporal change (first-order temporal derivative of color), while motion direction change is a less-salient second-order property, a change in the direction of change. This hypothesis does not exclude the possibility that processing latency differences affect perceptual asynchrony when it affects time markers. In agreement with this hypothesis, color–motion asynchrony is not accompanied by a corresponding difference in perceptual latency when the latency is estimated from cortical responses or behavioral reaction time (Amano et al.,
2007; Nishida & Johnston,
2002), and second-order temporal changes appear delayed relative to first-order temporal changes regardless of the stimulus attributes involved (Nishida & Johnston,
2002). The time marker hypothesis however cannot account for the small asynchrony between color and orientation (Zeki,
2003; Zeki & Moutoussis,
1997). Furthermore, the neural correlates of time marker processing remain unspecified.