A number of strategies have been developed to isolate these high-level aftereffects. For example, higher order shape aftereffects can be distinguished from conventional figural aftereffects because they occur even with very brief presentations, show stronger transfer across different retinal locations, and show less dependence on stimulus contrast (Suzuki,
2005). The latter effects are consistent with larger receptive fields and greater contrast invariance of extrastriate areas. Similarly, face aftereffects show strong transfer across changes in position and image size (Afraz & Cavanagh,
2008; Leopold et al.,
2001; Zhao & Chubb,
2001) and also across changes in orientation. For example, Watson and Clifford (
2003) found that adaptation to a distorted face depended on the axis of distortion within the object (e.g., a horizontal constriction of the face) regardless of the axis within the visual frame (e.g., whether the face was tilted clockwise or counterclockwise). Face aftereffects have also been found to depend more strongly on the perceptual category of the stimuli rather than their structural similarity. Thus, in a morph between male and female faces, separate contingent aftereffects can be generated for distortions that fall on different sides of the identity or gender boundary (i.e., when one face appears male and the other female) but not for equivalent physical differences within each category (Bestelmeyer et al.,
2008; Rotshtein, Henson, Treves, Driver, & Dolan,
2005). The adaptation is also more dependent on awareness compared to simpler stimuli, so that the adaptation is largely abolished when the face is masked by binocular suppression (Moradi, Koch, & Shimojo,
2005). Like motion aftereffects, aftereffects for faces have been reported following visualization (DeBruine, Welling, & Jones,
2010; Ganis & Schendan,
2008; Ryu, Borrmann, & Chaudhuri,
2008) and can also transfer across different representations of implied attributes. For example, adaptation to female or male headless bodies induces a gender aftereffect in the perception of a face (Ghuman, McDaniel, & Martin,
2010; though a similar transfer has not been found between the gender cues carried by faces and hands (Kovacs et al.,
2006)). On the other hand, it is unlikely that face aftereffects can always arise at a conceptual level, for adaptation to facial expressions does not occur for non-facial images or words that convey an emotion (Fox & Barton,
2007). Taken together, such results strongly suggest that aftereffects for high-level attributes do at least partly reflect response changes at high levels of visual processing.