Figure 4 shows the percentage of “right” responses as a function of Vernier Position and Cue Configuration for the three Cue Shapes. The data were analyzed with a 3 (Cue Shape) × 2 (Cue Configuration) × 5 (Vernier Position) repeated measures ANOVA using the Greenhouse–Geisser correction when appropriate. The analysis showed no main effect of Cue Shape on the percentage of “right” responses,
F(2, 22) < 1. As expected, there was a significant main effect of Cue Configuration (
F (1, 11) = 19.14,
p = 0.001,
η p 2 = 0.64), with the mean percentage of “right” responses being higher in the upper left and lower right configuration (61.7%) than in the upper right and lower left configuration (32.2%). This result replicates the standard repulsion effect found in previous studies using the method of constant stimuli (Pratt & Arnott,
2008). There was a main effect of Vernier Position,
F(1.14, 12.50) = 57.26,
p < 0.001,
η p 2 = 0.84, with the percentage of “right” responses increasing as the top vernier was increasingly offset to the right. This result would be expected regardless of any repulsion effect and demonstrates that participants were sensitive to physical changes in the position of the lines.
In addition, the analysis revealed both significant Cue Shape × Vernier Position (F(2.34, 25.73) = 3.48, p = 0.04, η p 2 = 0.24) and Cue Configuration × Vernier Position (F(4, 44) = 20.61, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.65) interactions. The Cue Shape × Cue Configuration interaction (F(1.29, 14.21) = 1.04, p = 0.19) and the three-way interaction (F(2.89, 31.76) = 1.08, p = 0.37) were not significant.
The main effects of Position and Cue Configuration replicate the basic attentional repulsion effect, and the Cue Configuration × Vernier Position interaction appears to arise from a larger repulsion effect for smaller vernier offsets than for larger ones. However, the Cue Shape × Vernier Position interaction suggests that manipulating the shape of the cue did change pattern of responses. Inspection of
Figure 4 suggests that this interaction may result from a steeper slope in the function relating vernier offset to the percentage of “right” responses in Condition 3 relative to the two other conditions.
To examine the possibility that the ARE may also be reduced in Condition 3, difference scores in the percentage of “right” responses between the two cue configurations (upper left/lower right minus upper right/lower left) were calculated as an estimate of the size of the repulsion effect, leaving two factors in the analysis: Cue Shape and Vernier Position. We ran two post-hoc comparisons—the first between Condition 2 (large circle) and Condition 3 (large oval), and the second between Condition 1 (small circle) and Condition 2, using the Sidak–Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (α S–B = 0.025). Comparisons between Conditions 2 and 3 revealed a significant main effect of Cue Shape on the difference score, F(1, 11) = 7.73, p = 0.02, η p 2 = 0.41. The mean difference score was smaller in Condition 3 (19.9%) than in Condition 2 (36.0%), suggesting that the repulsion effect was reduced when the cue contour bounded the vernier on the opposite side. Post-hoc comparisons were also run between Conditions 1 and 2 to examine the effects of cue size on the magnitude of the repulsion effect. The analysis showed no main effect of Cue Shape on the difference score F(1, 11) < 1. As expected, there was a main effect of Vernier Position, F(4, 44) = 10.52, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.49. The Cue Shape × Vernier Position interaction was not significant, F(4, 44) < 1. Thus, increasing the cue size while keeping the center of mass constant did not appear to change the magnitude of the repulsion effect despite the fact that the closest cue contour was closer to the position of the vernier lines in Condition 2. However, changing the contours of the cue to encompass the vernier reduced the magnitude of the repulsion effect to some extent but did not eliminate it.