For this reason, we propose that non-retinotopic feature integration requires the maintenance of information about motion streams in memory and the ability of attention to select and transfer this information to decision processes (see also, Kahneman, Treisman, & Gibbs,
1992; Pylyshyn,
1989; Yantis,
1992). At one extreme, one can assume that all lines are stored in visual short-term memory for temporally extended processing and their offsets can be selected by attention according to the task instruction. However, as discussed before, experimental evidence suggests that attention cannot individually access lines that are grouped within one stream. For example, attention cannot “ignore” a flank-offset presented in the attended stream. For this reason, we propose the following hierarchical two stage “grouping-attention” model of non-retinotopic feature integration (see also, Scharnowski, Hermens, Kammer, Öğmen, & Herzog,
2007). Motion grouping mechanisms “sort” single elements into various basic groups (i.e., the motion streams in our experiments). When grouping of the single elements is ambiguous, certain elements may belong to more than one group (e.g., the central line may belong to the left and the right motion stream, see
Figure 1). Attention cannot “access” the single lines in the basic groups. However, attention can operate on these groups and combine them into higher-order groups depending on task demands. Features are then integrated according to the selection of groups. For example, the offsets of the two central streams are integrated (by default) when attention is not focused selectively on either stream (
Figure 7). When observers are instructed to attend to one of the central streams only, the offset in the other stream does not, or only marginally, contribute to performance (
Figure 8). Hence, attention guides optional feature integration across streams.