The physical laws that govern the world cause objects to behave in a predictable way. For example, when one mobile object collides with another of approximately equal mass, the second one will move in a predictable way. This reaction is an example of physical causality, where the action of one object causes the reaction of another. Human adults (Michotte,
1946/1963; Scholl & Tremoulet,
2000), as well as young children and babies (Gopnik, Sobel, Schulz, & Glymour,
2001; Leslie & Keeble,
1987; Saxe & Carey,
2006), are able to perceive causality and are highly sensitive to violations of it. For a simple collision event, such violations include temporal or spatial gaps at the collision, and motion angles or speed ratios that seem to be at odds with the laws of physics (Michotte,
1946/1963; Schlottmann & Anderson,
1993; Schlottmann & Shanks,
1992; Straube & Chatterjee,
2010; Young, Rogers, & Beckmann,
2005). The perception of causality holds across different cultures (Morris & Peng,
1994), though it may be altered or impaired in certain neurological or psychiatric conditions such as autism (Ray & Schlottmann,
2007) and schizophrenia (Tschacher & Kupper,
2006). Evidence suggests that other primate species (Hauser & Spaulding,
2006; O'Connell & Dunbar,
2005), as well as corvids (Chappell,
2006; Seed, Tebbich, Emery, & Clayton,
2006), might be able to perceive and even reason based on causality as well (but see Penn & Povinelli,
2007).