Night myopia is a tendency for eyes to become near-sighted in dim illumination. Astronomers were the first to describe this phenomenon (Levene,
1965) as a need for correcting lenses of negative power to improve viewing of the stars. The phenomenon gained considerable importance during the Second World War because of the crucial need to visually detect points of light at sea or in the night sky (Otero, Plaza, & Salaverri,
1949; Otero & Duran,
1943). More recently it has been suggested that night myopia is a potential hindrance to safe driving at night (Charman,
1996; Cohen et al.,
2007; Fejer,
1995).
The simplest explanation of night myopia is that uncorrected myopia (or deliberate under-correction of myopia produced by maximum-plus refractions (Borish,
1970) is less noticeable during the day when high levels of ambient luminance reduce the size of the eye's pupil, thereby reducing the amount of blur on the retina (Charman,
1996). When ambient luminance declines, the pupil dilates, and retinal blur becomes noticeable subjectively; thus,the eye appears to have become nearsighted when, in fact, it was always nearsighted, but the manifestations of myopia had not been noticed. Although this explanation may account for Rayleigh's original description of night myopia (Rayleigh,
1883), and for much of the clinical incidence of night myopia in the general public (Charman,
1996), more sophisticated optical explanations are required to account for evidence obtained in carefully controlled laboratory experiments. One such explanation is based on the fact that most eyes have positive spherical aberration (SA) when accommodation is relaxed (Salmon & van de Pol,
2006). Like defocus, the blurring effect of SA is greatest when the pupil is large, a conditionwhich implies the visual effects of SA will be most noticeable under dim illumination conditions.
Night myopia might also be an artifact of increased accommodation to compensate for the increased blurring effects of SA when the pupil dilates. Positive ocular SA declines during accommodation (Young,
1801; Ivanoff,
1947; Lopez-Gil, Fernández-Sánchez, Legras, Montes-Mico, Lara, & Nguyen-Khoa,
2008; Lopez-Gil & Fernández-Sánchez,
2010) and therefore vision for distant objects benefits by viewing through a negative lens (or misfocusing a telescope) to stimulate accommodation (Ivanoff,
1947; Otero, & Duran,
1943). In this case preference for a negative viewing lens is misinterpreted as a sign of myopia. A similar misinterpretation might occur for presbyopic eyes with significant amounts of positive spherical aberration since retinal image quality for point sources will improve when viewing through a weak negative lens. (Mahajan,
1991).
All of the aforementioned explanations for night myopia refer to foveal vision under photopic conditions. When ambient illumination is reduced to mesopic levels, accommodation becomes less accurate and eventually vanishes in the scotopic domain where cones are no longer active (Campbell,
1953; Johnson,
1976). Moreover, as ambient light levels decline, the eye assumes a resting state (dark focus) for which its focusing power is somewhat greater than when viewing distant objects at higher luminances (Johnson,
1976) and thus the eye appears to have become relatively more myopic (Owens & Leibowitz,
1976; Simonelli & Roscoe,
1979; Braddick, Ayling, Sawyer, & Atkinson,
1981; Epstein,
1983; Kotulak, Morse, & Rabin,
1995). The Purkinje shift may also contribute to the night myopia phenomenon under scotopic illumination. If the scotopic refractive state is measured at the wavelength of peak sensitivity of rod photoreceptors (504 nm), then ocular chromatic aberration will make the scotopic eye appear relatively myopic compared to a photopic measurement at the peak of photopic sensitivity (555 nm).
Although optical instruments may be used to measure objectively the refractive state of the eye, most of the published evidence for night myopia was obtained by subjective procedures of the kind used routinely by clinical optometrists (Bohman & Saladin,
1980; Cohen et al.,
2007; Fejer,
1995; Leibowitz, Gish, & Sheehy,
1988). Yet none of the mechanistic explanations for night myopia reviewed above makes particular reference to the visual stimulus or the visual task used to assess subjectively the focus state of the eye. This omissionis surprising, given that optimum focus depends on spatial frequency in normally aberrated eyes (Koomen, Scolnik, & Tousey,
1951; Green & Campbell,
1965; Charman, Jennings, & Whitefoot,
1978). If different visual targets are used for subjective determination of refractive error during daytime and nighttime viewing, then differences in spatial frequency content of those targets might account, at least partially, for night myopia. Choice of visual target might also explain the failure of some experiments to elicit the phenomenon (Arumi, Chauhan, & Charman,
1997) which has given night myopia a reputation for being an enigmatic topic resting on a controversial foundation.
In an attempt to resolve some of the controversy surrounding night-myopia, we examined the importance of stimulus configuration when measuring ocular refractive state. We found that under photopic conditions the eye's refractive state is significantly more myopic when the eye's focus is optimized for detecting a point source on a dark background compared to the focus needed to optimize legibility of black letters on a white background. Since isolated point sources are more likely encountered at night, whereas extended objects are more likely encountered in the daytime, our results suggest that a significant part of the night myopia phenomenon is determined by the nature of the visual stimulus and the visual task used to assess ocular refractive state.