Much of the previous literature has found that our attention is automatically drawn to negative information more strongly than it is automatically drawn to positive information (Hansen & Hansen,
1988; Pratto & John,
1991), although emotion-induced enhancements for positive stimuli have also been found (e.g., Schupp, Junghofer, Weike, & Hamm,
2003). The “automatic” attention to negative information has been termed a “negativity effect” (or “negativity hypothesis”), and attention to positive information has been termed a “positivity effect.” For example, Kissler and Keil (
2008) found that when instructed to make a saccade toward a picture in the right peripheral visual field, facilitation occurred only for pleasant pictures and saccadic reaction times toward unpleasant pictures were slowed. Similarly, the negativity effect can be seen in event-related potentials (Ito, Larsen, Smith, & Cacioppo,
1998; Smith, Cacioppo, Larsen, & Chartrand,
2003), faster reaction to subliminally presented negative stimuli (Dijksterhuis & Aarts,
2003), and increased skin conductance responses even when presented subliminally (30 ms of exposure) and backwardly masked (see Öhman & Mineka,
2001). According to the aforementioned evolutionary theory, the faster detection of negative stimuli may have developed as a survival mechanism. The frequently found negativity effect may occur because negative stimuli such as a deadly poisonous spider require more immediate action (and thus faster processing) than, for example, a cute cuddly kitten, which poses little threat. In this sense, evaluation of threat may be a key underlying component of this evolutionary system (Öhman,
1993; Öhman et al.,
2001). Öhman (
1993) suggested that threat information is first processed by a feature detection system, which “tags” the stimuli as ancestrally or behaviorally relevant and passes the information to the organism's arousal system, which optimizes selective attention and orienting. Individuals then compare the stimuli with earlier memories and respond appropriately. However, many fears are irrational, and although they may have evolutional relevance, the individual may never have had an actual bad experience with the feared stimulus and thus have no associated negative memories to compare it to. Despite this criticism, evidence for rapid processing of fear stimuli has been found. Fear-relevant targets among neutral distracters are detected faster than neutral targets among fear-relevant distracters (Öhman et al.,
2001). However, this was only true if the target was feared by that individual—no effect was present for generic fear-relevant stimuli if they were not feared by the viewer. This raises the question of why there are so many individual differences in feared stimuli—if this system is based on evolutionary dangers, everyone should hold the same fears. Furthermore, these individual differences cannot be completely explained by fears developed through bad experience because many phobias are irrational.