Despite the existence of different theoretical frameworks, the common idea is that attributes of the relevant events in the visual scene depend on a specific, though context dependent, time register before reaching consciousness (Aymoz & Viviani,
2004). By contrasting passive and active conditions, other authors have suggested that endogenous activity may also contribute to sensory binding. Haggard, Clark, and Kalogeras (
2002), for instance, showed that performing a voluntary action influences the temporal perception of sensory events associated with that action. In their study, the perception of a sound was shifted 46 ms earlier in time relative to a baseline condition when it was preceded by a voluntary motor action. Interpreted within the framework of intentional binding by the authors, this effect underlines a contribution of “predictive models of motor control in constructing conscious experience of action.” Indeed, voluntary actions imply predictive mechanisms which are used to anticipate expected sensory consequences of acting, evaluate action feasibility (Jeannerod,
2006), and also guide actual motor behavior (Wolpert, Ghahramani, & Jordan,
1995). These predictive mechanisms are thought to influence the temporal perception of sensory and motor events (Haggard et al.,
2002; Stetson, Cui, Montague, & Eagleman,
2006; Wenke & Haggard,
2009). Consequently, the perception of different object attributes is also expected to be influenced by whether or not these attributes vary in relation to voluntary motor action, particularly when they are processed with different latencies (Nowak & Bullier,
1997). In agreement with this, previous studies have shown that the temporal constraints associated with object color and position processing are affected by the observational context. For instance, Aymoz and Viviani (
2004) showed that when embedding visual changes into an observed biological movement, the perception of object displacement did not lag the perception of color change as it is usually found in perceptual tasks. Thus, in the action condition the perceived temporal synchronization of attribute changes may not exclusively result from relative time delays associated with the neural organization of the visual system (Nowak & Bullier,
1997) or perceptual anticipation due to prediction of the visual event since the reduction of temporal asynchrony was not observed when the object displacement resulted from the impact of a nonbiological stimulus (Aymoz & Viviani,
2004). The authors' interpretation of this effect was that the moving hand calls into play the interaction between the visual and the motor system. It is indeed acknowledged that observed human actions are usually interpreted within a neural network similar to the one used for planning and executing actual motor actions (Grezes, Costes, & Decety,
1998). Generating a reliable representation of a human action may then provide an access to predicted sensory consequences of acting, and thus may contribute to the binding mechanism whereby all attributes of the relevant events in the visual scene are set into strict time register before reaching consciousness (Moore & Haggard,
2008). As a consequence of predictive motor mechanisms, the neural timing mechanism could be modulated during the action execution period in order to facilitate the processing and integration of action-related sensory signals (Wenke & Haggard,
2009). In case of variable delays in sensory and motor pathways, the temporal relationship between action and sensations is thought to be recalibrated to overcome changing latencies and to restore both the perception of object unity (Yarrow, Haggard, Heal, Brown, & Rothwell,
2001) and perceived events causality (Stetson et al.,
2006) or agency (Haggard et al.,
2002).