While the retention of color information in short-term perceptual memory undoubtedly provides us with a great deal of information about objects and surfaces within our environment, what do the experimental results reported here tell us about how this information is organized in short-term perceptual memory? If we look at results from the spatial frequency domain, the bandwidths of tuning revealed by memory masking experiments have been found to be of the order of approximately ±1 octave (Magnussen et al.,
1991; Nemes, Whitaker, Heron, & McKeefry,
2011). This value is remarkably similar to estimates of bandwidths for spatial frequency channels that exist in low-level sensory visual processing, revealed by sensory masking and contrast adaptation studies (Blakemore & Campbell,
1969; Blakemore & Nachmias,
1971; Blakemore, Nachmias, & Sutton,
1970; Campbell & Robson,
1968; Georgeson & Harris,
1984). This correspondence has been taken as evidence to support the idea that there is close association between the sensory mechanisms that are involved in the low-level visual processing of spatial frequency and those that are involved in the storage of this information in perceptual or sensory memory (see Pasternak & Greenlee,
2005 for a review). Following the same rationale, can the tuning revealed by the color memory masking experiments in this study be linked with the sensory processing of color in the visual system? We know that, following photon capture by the retinal photoreceptors, color information is signaled by cone-opponent mechanisms (DeValois et al.,
1966). These mechanisms rely upon linear combinations of L-, M-, and S-cone inputs and predominate in the subcortical and early stages of cortical color processing. Cone-opponent mechanisms exhibit responses to color stimuli that vary sinusoidally across DKL color space (Derrington et al.,
1984; DeValois, Cottaris et al.,
2000).
Figure 8a plots data recorded from such a cone-opponent (−L + M) neuron recorded from the monkey LGN (DeValois, DeValois et al.,
2000). Its response is plotted as a function of chromatic axis in DKL color space and has been fitted by a Gaussian function that provides an estimate of bandwidth (
σ). This gives a value of approximately 60°, typical of the bandwidth estimates for linear chromatic mechanisms found in subcortical and early V1 color processing (DeValois, Cottaris et al.,
2000; D'Zmura & Knoblauch,
1998). This bandwidth, however, is considerably wider than those revealed in our memory masking experiments. Therefore, it seems unlikely that broadly tuned, linear cone-opponent mechanisms mediate these interference effects. An alternative basis for these effects might lie in the fact that rather than adhering to two cone-opponent or cardinal mechanisms, color processing within the cerebral cortex instead relies upon multiple “higher order” chromatic mechanisms that are tuned to many different directions in color space (Clifford et al.,
2003; DeValois, Cottaris et al.,
2000; DeValois, DeValois et al.,
2000; DeValois et al.,
1997; Goda & Fujii,
2001; Krauskopf et al.,
1986; Li & Lennie,
1997; McGraw et al.,
2004; Webster & Mollon,
1991; Zaidi & Halevy,
1993). These higher order mechanisms appear to be the result of recombinations of outputs from the cone-opponent mechanisms (see Eskew,
2009 for a review). Currently, there is some debate as to whether they arise as the result of linear or non-linear interactions. Close to threshold, higher order mechanisms have been found to be largely linear with bandwidths of approximately 60°, similar to those exhibited by cone-opponent mechanisms (D'Zmura & Knoblauch,
1998; Giulianini & Eskew,
1998; Hansen & Gegenfurtner,
2006; Sankeralli & Mullen,
1997). On the other hand, at suprathreshold levels, there is evidence to suggest varying degrees of non-linearity in the formation of these higher order chromatic mechanisms, which leads to the generation of more narrowly tuned color mechanisms with bandwidths of the order of 30–40° (Clifford et al.,
2003; Goda & Fujii,
2001; McKeefry et al.,
2004). These values are in closer accord with tuning characteristics revealed by the memory masking experiments in this study. Thus, we might speculate that the chromatic information utilized by short-term perceptual memory is derived from a stage in color processing beyond that where the transformation from linear, broadband, cone-opponent processing to non-linear, more narrowband higher order chromatic processing has taken place. Narrowly tuned chromatic mechanisms are significant in color processing in that they provide a potential link with specific hues—something that broadband mechanisms do not offer. In order for more broadly tuned mechanisms to signal stimulus color, it would require another stage at which their outputs could be compared (Eskew,
2009). Outputs from narrowly tuned chromatic mechanisms, on the other hand, could directly signal stimulus hue. This link raises the possibility that color information in short-term perceptual memory is organized around perceptual color categories. A similar suggestion has been made in the light of experimental findings that have demonstrated that the extent of degradation in the fidelity of remembered colors is less marked for more perceptually relevant or focal colors (Berlin & Kay,
1969; Heider,
1972; Nemes, Parry, & McKeefry,
2010). Also consistent with this idea is the fact that hue naming functions, which were used in preliminary experiments in this study to define the reference stimuli, have bandwidths that are similar to those obtained from the color memory masking experiments. In
Figure 8b, blue (
p[blue]) and yellow (
p[yellow]) hue naming functions have been plotted as a function of chromatic axis. The values of
σ obtained from the Gaussian fits reveal bandwidths of 41.8° for blue and 29.1° for yellow hue naming functions, comparable not only to those values revealed by color memory masking in this study but also to those reported for non-linear, higher order chromatic mechanisms. Thus, there is some circumstantial evidence to suggest that color categories may form the basis for the storage of chromatic information in perceptual memory. However, there were discrepancies in terms of the bandwidth estimates between the memory masking and the hue naming data in the case of red and green stimuli. The hue naming functions (not shown) for red (
p[red]) and green (
p[green]) are much broader, exhibiting plateaus across a range of color space (see also DeValois et al.,
1997). This discrepancy may be a consequence of the fact that the hue naming method we employed restricted observer responses to only four basic color categories (red, green, blue, and yellow) to describe the stimuli. There is the possibility, therefore, that these broader categories might consist of further subcategories with narrower bandwidths that may be more in keeping with the results revealed by memory masking. Certainly, further work will be required to establish more rigorously whether perceptual color categories form the basis around which short-term perceptual memory for color is organized.