There have been several previous studies of the effect of eccentricity on contrast sensitivity (e.g., Pöppel & Harvey,
1973; Hilz & Cavonius,
1974; Koenderink, Bouman, Bueno de Mesquita, & Slappendel,
1978a,
1978b,
1978c,
1978d; Rovamo, Virsu, & Näsänen,
1978; Rovamo & Virsu,
1979; Rijsdijk, Kroon, & van der Wildt,
1980; Robson & Graham,
1981; Wright & Johnston,
1983; Kelly,
1984; Johnston,
1987; Pointer & Hess,
1989; Rovamo, Franssila, & Näsänen,
1992; Foley, Varadharajan, Koh, & Farias,
2007; Hess, Baker, May, & Wang,
2008). The typical finding in these previous studies is a linear decline in log contrast sensitivity as a patch of target grating is shifted further from the fovea. Several of these studies also investigated spatial frequency effects (e.g., Robson & Graham,
1981; Pointer & Hess,
1989) and found that the sensitivity functions were vertical translations of each other when eccentricity was expressed in terms of stimulus carrier cycles (rather than degrees of visual angle). Another established phenomenon is the
horizontal–vertical anisotropy, where there is a shallower decline in contrast sensitivity along the horizontal meridian compared to the vertical meridian. In addition, a
vertical-meridian anisotropy has been identified where sensitivity is greater in the inferior meridian compared to the superior meridian (see Abrams, Nizam, & Carrasco,
2012 for data and a review).