Evidence of expression-independent identity aftereffects in 8-year-old children refutes the hypothesis that children make more errors than adults on face recognition tasks because their representation of identity is not expression-invariant. Indeed, these data add to a growing body of evidence that many of the requisite skills underlying adult expertise are present by 8 years of age, including holistic processing (e.g., de Heering et al.,
2007), norm-based coding (e.g., Anzures et al.,
2009; Jeffery et al.,
2011) and expression-independent representations of identity (current study). Nonetheless, performance on a variety of face perception tasks continues to improve throughout childhood (see Jeffery et al.,
2011 for a review) indicating that adult-like expertise is slow to develop. The results of the current study contribute to a growing body of evidence that it may be most prudent to view the development of face perception during childhood as a process of refinement. For example, although infants and young children show sensitivity to feature shape and spacing (e.g., Hayden, Bhatt, Reed, Corbly, & Joseph,
2007; Gilchrist & McKone,
2003), they are less sensitive than adults to variations in these characteristics (Baudouin et al.,
2010; Gilchrist & McKone,
2003; Mondloch et al.,
2002). Likewise, although 8-year-old children seem to represent faces in a multi-dimensional face space, they are less sensitive than adults to variation within dimensions (Anzures et al.,
2009; Jeffery et al.,
2010), and they rely almost exclusively on a single dimension (eye color) when judging face similarity (Nishimura et al.,
2009). Likewise, it is quite possible that despite having an expression-independent representation of identity as revealed in the current study, children have difficulty ignoring changes in expression in identity tasks. Children are less able than adults to selectively attend to identity when expression varies in a Garner interference task (Baudouin et al.,
2008; but see Spangler et al.,
2010). They also make more errors than adults when asked to recognize facial identity across changes in expression (Mondloch, Geldart et al.,
2003; see also Bruce et al.,
2000). Our results suggest that these findings cannot be attributed to children failing to form an expression-independent representation of identity. Rather, Garner interference effects may reflect a failure of selective attention, something that may be augmented when children are asked to recognize unfamiliar faces across changes in expression in a delayed match-to-sample task.