We examined the visual system's use of line junction cues in the perception of 3D structure. In
Experiment 1, we found that adults perceive a concave wire frame as convex when it is viewed monocularly. In
Experiment 2, we investigated whether 5- and 7-month-old infants also perceive the center of the wire frame as convex. Both 5- and 7-month-old infants reached more to the center region of the display in the monocular condition than the binocular condition suggesting that they, like adults, perceived the wire frame as convex when binocular cues were eliminated.
The results of this study are consistent with previous findings that infants use constraints in other domains, including the perception of shape from shading (Granrud, Yonas, & Opland,
1985), perception of distance from linear perspective, and texture gradient cues (Arterberry, Yonas, & Bensen,
1989; Hemker et al.,
2010; Yonas, Granrud, Arterberry, & Hanson,
1986). In addition, a recent study using a method similar to that used in
Experiment 2 has shown that 6-month-old infants respond to the hollow face illusion. That is, infants perceive a concave mask as convex, demonstrating their use of the constraint that faces are convex or a general constraint that objects tend to be convex (Corrow, Granrud, Mathison, & Yonas,
in press). This brings up the possibility that infants and adults may, in addition, use other constraints to interpret the internal Y-shaped vertex as convex (e.g., a general convexity assumption).
The finding that 5-month-old infants are sensitive to line junction information for depth is particularly interesting considering that most reaching studies investigating pictorial depth cues have found an increase in responsiveness to these cues between 5 and 7 months. However, the results of the present study are consistent with several recent reports that infants as young as 5 months of age respond to pictorial depth cues (Arterberry,
2008; Hemker et al.,
2010; Kavsek et al.,
2009).
Future work should examine whether the constraints used to perceive depth from line junctions and other depth cues are built-in or learned. Infants younger than 5 months of age were not tested in the present study due to limitations in their ability to reach. However, it is possible that younger infants would respond to line junction cues if different measures were used. With a sensitive enough measure, extremely young infants might demonstrate the use of line junction constraints to interpret an ambiguous 2D image on the retina. With the ability to test younger infants, we may be able to answer the central question of whether sensitivity to line junction information is learned over the history of the child or evolutionary history.