An important set of examples is provided by several experimental accounts demonstrating how displacement information affects timing computations. These studies show that timing estimates of moving stimuli are proportional in size to stimulus velocity (Brown,
1995; Goldstone & Lhamon,
1974; Kanai, Paffen, Hogendoorn, & Verstraten,
2006; Roelofs & Zeeman,
1951). Faster stimuli are overestimated with respect to slower and stationary stimuli (
time dilation). A subset of these studies reveal two important pieces of information: (a) Timing biases are best explained by a
change-based account of timekeeping performance (i.e., subjective passage of time is indexed by the number of physical changes occurring within an objective time frame) and (b) Motion—a continuous change of space—acts as an important stimulus duration cue (Kanai et al.,
2006). Kanai et al. (
2006) attempted to isolate the source of this effect by dissecting visual motion into its physical constituents: motion trajectory and coherence, spatial frequency, temporal frequency, and velocity, given by the ratio of these two latter variables. Throughout a series of experiments, the authors established that the critical factor subtending the illusion was represented by temporal frequency (hertz, or rate of change in time), as the time dilation effect could be even observed in a stationary flickering Gaussian blob. A similar study was also recently conducted by Kaneko & Murakami (
2009) using slightly different stimulus configurations (horizontally drifting Gabor-patch stimuli vs. concentric expanding gratings used by Kanai et al.) to address the quantitative relationship between stimulus motion and stimulus apparent duration. These latter authors observed that stimulus velocity (temporal frequency/spatial frequency ratio) provided the best estimator of stimulus duration overestimations. This led the authors to determine the visual processing pathway area MT as the most likely neuroanatomical candidate for the time dilation effect, since it hosts neuronal subpopulations selectively tuned to stimulus speed (Kaneko & Murakami,
2009; Perrone & Thiele,
2001; Priebe, Lisberger, & Movshon,
2006). Despite these slightly different conclusions, presumably due to the adoption of different stimulus configurations (Kaneko & Murakami,
2009), both studies frame results in accordance to a change-based interpretation of time indexing. Time dilation relates more in general to the
filled-duration illusion, in which the number of changes delimiting a period of stimulation affects its overall perceived duration.