We computed the proportion of trials each observer reported bouncing in each of the 10 conditions. These figures served as the units for our statistical analyses described in the following text. The group mean proportions of reported bounces as a function of texture density difference between the targets in the presence and absence of an auditory tone at the point of coincidence are shown in
Figure 2. It is clear from the figure that auditory-induced bounce resolutions dominate when the targets had identical texture density (0% texture density difference), replicating previous studies that have employed identical targets. As the texture density differences increased, the proportion of reported bounces decreased. Nevertheless, the proportion of trials in which bouncing was reported was higher in the sound condition than the no-sound condition at each texture density difference. Crucially for the current study, these results demonstrate that the auditory-induced bias toward bouncing persists despite modifications rendering the targets visually distinguishable resulting in the motion sequences unequivocally consistent with streaming, generalizing Grove and Sakurai's (
2009) observations to motion sequences with dynamic random-element stimuli.
To formally explore these findings, we conducted a two (sound [absent, present]) × five (texture density difference [0%, 10%, 20%, 30%, 40%]) within participants' analysis of variance (ANOVA) with proportion of trials for which bounces were reported as the repeated measure and found a significant effect for sound, F1,18 = 24.9, p < 0.01; a significant effect for texture density difference, F4,72 = 35.4, p < 0.01, indicating that the proportion of bounces decreased as texture density differences increased. A significant interaction between sound and texture, F4,72 = 7.6, p < 0.01, was found, reflecting the reduction in the effectiveness of the sound to promote bouncing above the no-sound condition as the targets became more visually distinct. This argues against a simple sound-induced additive bias, as pointed out by one reviewer. Paired comparisons (using the Bonferroni correction for multiple t-tests, α = 0.01) between the sound and no-sound conditions at each level of texture density differences revealed that the proportions of bounce responses were nevertheless significantly higher in the sound condition than the no-sound condition at all texture density differences (0% difference: no sound [M = 0.34; SD = 0.34]; sound [M = 0.76; SD = 0.28], t18 = 4.05, p < 0.01; 10% difference: no sound [M = 0.2; SD = 0.24]; sound [M = 0.63; SD = 0.29], t18 = 4.21, p < 0.01; 20% difference: no sound [M = 0.08; SD = 0.2]; sound [M = 0.36; SD = 0.3], t18 = 3.27, p < 0.01; 30% difference: no sound [M = 0.05; SD = 0.17]; sound [M = 0.26; SD = 0.28], t18 = 3.73, p < 0.01; 40% difference: no sound [M = 0.05; SD = 0.2]; sound [M = 0.17; SD = 0.25], t18 = 3.02, p < 0.01).