Free
Article  |   June 2012
A new spectrally sharpened sensor basis to predict color naming, unique hues, and hue cancellation
Author Affiliations
  • Javier Vazquez-Corral
    Computer Science Department, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona and Computer Vision Center, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain
    javier.vazquez@cvc.uab.cat
  • J. Kevin O'Regan
    Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception - CNRS UMR 8158, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France
    jkevin.oregan@gmail.com
  • Maria Vanrell
    Computer Science Department, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona and Computer Vision Center, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain
    maria.vanrell@cvc.uab.cat
  • Graham D. Finlayson
    School of Computing Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom
    G.Finlayson@uea.ac.uk
Journal of Vision June 2012, Vol.12, 7. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/12.6.7
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Javier Vazquez-Corral, J. Kevin O'Regan, Maria Vanrell, Graham D. Finlayson; A new spectrally sharpened sensor basis to predict color naming, unique hues, and hue cancellation. Journal of Vision 2012;12(6):7. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.6.7.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract
Abstract
Abstract:

Abstract  When light is reflected off a surface, there is a linear relation between the three human photoreceptor responses to the incoming light and the three photoreceptor responses to the reflected light. Different colored surfaces have different linear relations. Recently, Philipona and O'Regan (2006) showed that when this relation is singular in a mathematical sense, then the surface is perceived as having a highly nameable color. Furthermore, white light reflected by that surface is perceived as corresponding precisely to one of the four psychophysically measured unique hues. However, Philipona and O'Regan's approach seems unrelated to classical psychophysical models of color constancy. In this paper we make this link. We begin by transforming cone sensors to spectrally sharpened counterparts. In sharp color space, illumination change can be modeled by simple von Kries type scalings of response values within each of the spectrally sharpened response channels. In this space, Philipona and O'Regan's linear relation is captured by a simple Land-type color designator defined by dividing reflected light by incident light. This link between Philipona and O'Regan's theory and Land's notion of color designator gives the model biological plausibility. We then show that Philipona and O'Regan's singular surfaces are surfaces which are very close to activating only one or only two of such newly defined spectrally sharpened sensors, instead of the usual three. Closeness to zero is quantified in a new simplified measure of singularity which is also shown to relate to the chromaticness of colors. As in Philipona and O'Regan's original work, our new theory accounts for a large variety of psychophysical color data.

Introduction
The human brain treats colors asymmetrically: as regards colored lights, certain hues of red, green, yellow, and blue, the so-called “unique hues” (Valberg, 2001), are perceived as being more “pure” than other hues; as regards colored surfaces, the well-known “World Color Survey” (WCS) shows that certain colors are considered more basic or prototypical or “focal” across different cultures (red, yellow, green, and blue being the four most frequent) (Berlin & Kay, 1969; Kay, 2005). 
Explanations for these facts cannot lie in the physical light spectra or reflectance properties, because these are continuous functions with no intrinsic asymmetries. Equally, appealing to the fact that there are three cone types does not in any obvious way by itself predict the experimentally observed asymmetries. Thus, explanations must be sought in neural (Parraga, Troscianko, & Tolhurst, 2002), environmental (Yendrikhovskij, 2001), cultural, or linguistic mechanisms (Kay & Regier, 2003). 
It seems at first sight that a plausible candidate for a neural mechanism might lie in the opponent color processing carried out by the early visual system (Hering, 1891; Wyszecki & Stiles, 1982). However, the extremes of the red/green and blue/yellow axes proposed in the opponent theory unfortunately do not correspond to the four unique hues. In particular, the theory would predict that to appear as unique green, a light should only activate the red/green channel, and not the blue/yellow channel. Yet it is found that to appear uniquely green a light must contain some yellow. Likewise it is found that to appear uniquely yellow, some red must be mixed in; to appear uniquely blue, some green must be mixed in (Burns, Elsner, Pokorny, & Smith, 1984; Cicerone, Krantz, & Larimer, 1975; Kuehni, 2004; Mollon & Jordan, 1997; Valberg, 2001; Webster, Miyahara, Malkoc, & Raker, 2000; Wuerger, Atkinson, & Cropper, 2005). 
Perceptual saliency of colors is another mechanism that has been invoked, this time to explain the intercultural data of the World Color Survey concerning the naming of surface colors. The idea is that speakers distribute color names over perceptual color space in a way to maximize the perceptual similarity between colors having the same names, and to minimize the similarity between colors having different names (Kay & Regier, 2003). Whereas this model does a reasonable job of explaining the boundaries between different color names, it provides no explanation for the exact hues which are considered to be “focal.” 
Recently, Philipona and O'Regan (2006) defined a new approach for explaining the unique hues and intercultural World Color Survey naming data. Philipona and O'Regan follow a very simple idea already implicit in the color literature: color should be considered to be the biological analogue of what physicists call surface reflectance. Philipona and O'Regan take the physicist's notion of reflectance, and create a biological reflectance function which can be used by the brain with its three broad-band sensors. They calculate this biological reflectance function for a wide variety of surfaces and discover that certain surfaces have the mathematical property of being “singular.” What this means is that these surfaces take incoming light, which usually can vary in a 3-dimensional space defined by L, M, and S cone activations, and transform it into light which varies only in either a 2- or in a 1-dimensional subspace of the LMS activation space. Because singular surfaces reduce variability from three dimensions to two or one dimension, they can be said to display a simpler behavior concerning how they affect incoming light than the majority of surfaces. What is now extremely striking is that these singular surfaces turn out to be almost exactly the red, yellow, green, and blue surfaces most frequently observed to be focal. Furthermore, if these surfaces are illuminated by a canonical source of natural illumination (Illuminant D65), the hues of the resulting reflected light correspond accurately to the monochromatic lights widely considered to be “unique” in psychophysical experiments. 
A strength of the Philipona and O'Regan approach is that it provides these explanations for both naming data and unique hues without any parameter adjustments. Well-known results on hue cancellation and hue equilibrium (Chichilnisky & Wandell, 1999; Jameson & Hurvich, 1955) also fall out very exactly from the predicted unique hue data. 
A weakness of the Philipona and O'Regan approach up until now has been the fact that no clear link has been made with classical approaches in color psychophysics, physiology, or artificial vision. In the present paper we remedy this by showing that the Philipona and O'Regan approach is closely related to the notion of spectral sharpening used in computational vision to obtain improved color constancy. The idea is that to characterize surface reflectivity in a way that is independent of illuminant, it is advantageous to use, not the normal three cone sensors of the human eye, but “sharpened” sensors which are linear combinations of the cone mechanisms. The sharp sensors allow us to define surface color “designators” similar to those suggested by Land in his retinex theory (1964). (Color designators are the color response for a surface divided by the color response to white.) The sharp designators defined by our approach have the property that they are much more independent of illumination than those defined in Land's approach. Furthermore, our sharp color designators turn out to be essentially the same as the biological reflectances defined by Philipona and O'Regan. Importantly, to compute the sharp designators we do not need to know what reflectance we are looking at: we simply divide the sharpened sensor responses of the (unknown) surface by the sharpened sensor responses of white. This is in contradistinction to the original Philipona and O'Regan theory in which biological reflectances can only be calculated once the transformation to virtual sensors particular to a given surface has been found. Removing the requirement that “we know what we are looking at” is a significant contribution of this paper. 
An additional contribution of our paper is to improve the measure of singularity used by Philipona and O'Regan. Our new measure provides an appealing, perceptually reasonable link to the notion of chromaticness, that is, the degree to which a light deviates from grey. We show that using our new measure of singularity, predictions of color naming and unique hues can be obtained that are at least as and possibly more precise than those obtained by Philipona and O'Regan. 
The paper is organized as follows. In section 2 we explain the biological model of Philipona and O'Regan. After this, in section 3 we define our model based on a set of sharp sensors and a new compact singularity index. This is followed by section 4 where we summarize the different results obtained. 
Philipona and O'Regan's biological model
Philipona and O'Regan's (2006) biological model is built on the assumption that the human vision system must attempt to extract the reflection properties of surfaces in the world independently of ambient lighting conditions. In other words, it must try to deliver a canonical, biological representation of reflectance. 
Physicists achieve this task by defining the notion of “reflectance function” linking incident light energy at a particular wavelength to reflected light energy at that wavelength. Because the majority of surfaces only absorb or reflect light energy at a given wavelength, and do not redistribute energy in other wavelengths, physicists can define reflectance at a given wavelength λ as a scalar s(λ) attenuation between 0 and 1, and write a simple linear relation linking incident light energy e(λ) at wavelength λ to reflected light energy p(λ) at that wavelength:  so that the physical reflectance of a surface is simply the ratio of reflected to incident light at each wavelength. 
Unlike physicists, who can measure energy at each monochromatic wavelength using a spectroradiometer, information accessible to the brain is blurred over the breadth of wavelengths that the human L, M, and S cone types are sensitive to. It is now no longer true that the effect of the surface on incident light can be expressed simply as an attenuation of energy within each of these broad bands. However, Philipona and O'Regan show that it is still possible to define a “biological” reflectance measure that links the information accessible to the brain about the incident light, to the information accessible to the brain about the reflected light in a way analogous to physicists' Equation 1
The information accessible to the brain about an illuminant e(λ) is the vector corresponding to the responses of the three cone types to that illuminant:  here t denotes the transpose of the vector, Qi(λ) for i = 1,2,3 define the absorption of the three human cone types at each wavelength λ, and we integrate over the visible spectrum ψ
The information accessible to the brain about the reflected light is the vector corresponding to the responses of the three cone types to the reflected light from the surface:  where S(λ) is the physicist's reflectance function for the surface s
Philipona and O'Regan now show the at first sight surprising result that for any surface s(λ) there exists a 3×3 matrix As which is independent of the illuminant e and very accurately describes the way the surface transforms the accessible information about any incident light into the accessible information about reflected light:   
As is the 3×3 matrix best taking ps,e (for any illuminant e) to we in a least-squares sense. Philipona and O'Regan studied the validity of such an equation for a very large number of natural and artificial illuminants, and for a very large number of colored surfaces. In fact, the result is analytically true if incoming illumination is of dimensionality 3, that is, if it can be described as a weighted sum of three basis functions (Philipona & O'Regan, 2006). Since this is known to be true to a good approximation for daylights (Judd et al., 1964), the equation is very accurate. 
Equation 4 is the biological analogue of the physicist's relation (1), but because it is written in terms of vectors and matrices instead of scalars, Philipona and O'Regan could not immediately invert it by dividing the vector ps,e by the vector we to obtain the biological equivalent of the physicist's reflectance in Equation 1. Philipona and O'Regan were able to do something similar however by first diagonalizing the matrix As, that is, writing it as the product (Ts)−1Ds Ts, where Ds is a diagonal matrix, and Ts is a transformation matrix. In that case Equation 4 becomes  so that   
Matrix Ts operating on ps,e and we maps these vectors into a basis where the accessible information matrix is diagonal. Because of the linearity of the integrals, the same effect can be achieved if instead of using the usual L, M, and S cones, we used a set of “virtual” sensors obtained precisely by taking this linear combination Ts of the cone responses:   
(Note we are using Greek letters to denote virtual sensors.) Then we can write, in terms of the virtual responses ρs,e and ωs,e:  Let us denote the ith component of the diagonal matrix Ds as ris giving   
Thus, by considering the virtual, recomposed sensors instead of the eye's actual LMS responses, Equation 10 defines a biological reflectance notion analogous to the physicist's reflectance defined in Equation 1 for each wavelength. For any surface, instead of having a reflectance function defined at every wavelength, we have a biological reflectance defined by three reflectance coefficients ris, each being the ratio of reflected to incident light within one of the three virtual wavelength bands defined for i = 1,2,3
Here we see the link with Retinex theory, in which Land (1964) proposed a similar definition of surface color. He called the sensor response triplet for light from a given surface divided by the triplet of responses for light from a perfect white reflectance (which is equivalent to taking the incident light itself) a color designator. The difference in Land's approach is that he used LMS responses, hoping that color designators would be approximately independent of illumination. Philipona and O'Regan, on the other hand, used responses of the recomposed virtual sensors defined for each surface by Ts
The Ts found by Philipona and O'Regan will typically map the cone sensor functions into virtual sensors which have more concentrated support in certain wavelength regions: they are LMS type sensors but appear spectrally sharper than the cones. Because of this property they will more nearly have the property that the associated color designators are independent of illumination. 
And here we see also the link to spectral sharpening. In spectral sharpening various algorithms are designed to make the reflectance term of Equation 10 as independent of illumination as possible (Chong, Gortler, & Zickler, 2007; Finlayson, Drew, & Funt, 1994b). However, unlike the Philipona and O'Regan work, which returns biological color reflectance terms using a different transformation matrix Ts for each surface, spectral sharpening seeks a single transformation for all surfaces and lights. One of the main contributions of this paper is to show that we can use a single, carefully chosen, transformation T and predict unique hue and color naming data equally well as the Philipona and O'Regan approach which used a per surface transformation Ts. Thus, and this is a significant improvement over the original work, we need not know the surface we are looking at in order to apply the theory. 
A second step in the Philipona and O'Regan formulation concerns their singularity index. Philipona and O'Regan calculated their biological reflectance coefficients for the set of Munsell chips used in the World Color Survey and noted that in certain cases, one or two of the three Philipona and O'Regan reflectance coefficients were close to zero. They called surfaces with this property “singular,” because they have the exceptional property of absorbing all light in one or two of the three bands defined by the virtual sensors. Such chips in some sense behave in a “simpler” fashion than other chips, because the variability of light reflected off them can be described within one or two bands, instead of needing three bands of light to be described, as is usually the case. An implication of the biological reflectance triple having two zeros (or two values close to 0) is that under different lights only one of the virtual responses changes. For example, a red surface with a biological reflectance triple of (1,0,0) implies a response under different lights of (k,0,0). That is, in the sense that variability is restricted to a plane or a line in 3-dimensional color space, one could say that such chips are in some sense more stable under changes of illumination, and might be easier to characterize, leading to their being more frequently designated as focal. 
Philipona and O'Regan's singularity index took the biological reflectance triple rs and sorted its elements in descending order. It then calculated the 2-vector βs:   
If k1 and k2 are respectively the maximum first and second beta components over a set of surfaces, Philipona and O'Regan's singularity index for a given surface was defined as:   
SPO is large when one or more of the Philipona and O'Regan biological reflectance components are relatively very small. Philipona and O'Regan's hypothesis was that large singularity would correspond to colors that would be likely to be given a focal name in a given culture. Indeed, Philipona and O'Regan showed that this was the case: a strong correlation was found between the SPO of Equation 12 and the frequency with which colors in the WCS dataset are considered prototypical in different cultures. Philipona and O'Regan also extended their analysis to the question of unique hues and demonstrated that the singularity index could predict the position of the wavelengths for unique hues found classically in color psychophysics. 
Spectral sharpening to define a more plausible biological model
The notion of spectral sharpening derives its roots from Land's retinex theory where there is a notion of color designator defined similarly to Philipona and O'Regan's notion of biological reflectance: The LMS triplet for an unknown surface under unknown light is divided by the response of a white surface (under the same light). In so doing the intent (or hope) is that the light should “cancel” and the color designator should be illuminant independent. However, designators calculated for the original cone sensors are not optimally illuminant independent. Thus the technique of Spectral Sharpening is used to find a single transform of cone responses with respect to which color designators are as independent of the illuminant as possible. Such sensors have sensitivities that are more narrowly concentrated and less overlapping in the visible spectrum than those of the original cones. Spectrally sharpened color designators are similar to Philipona and O'Regan's notion of biological reflectance, except that a unique transformation is used to create virtual responses, instead of having a different transform for each surface. 
Expressed formally, spectral sharpening (Finlayson, Drew, & Funt, 1994a) seeks to find a single surface-independent sharpening transform T such that over all surfaces s:  which implies  Note that, in contradistinction to Philipona and O'Regan, all surfaces share the same sharpening transform (no dependency on s). 
There is today a large amount of literature on how to find the best transform T. In (Finlayson et al., 1994a) the starting point for sharpening was exactly the Equation 14. There it was shown that if reflectance and illumination are respectively modeled by 2- and 3-dimensional linear models (or the converse), then Equation 14 holds exactly. This is a remarkable result in two respects. First, using the statistical analysis provided by Marimont and Wandell (1992) (that modeled light and reflectance by how they projected to form sensor responses) a 2-dimensional model for illumination and a 3-dimensional model for surface provides a tolerable model of real response data. Second, this result provides a strong theoretical argument for believing that a single sharp transform can be used for all surfaces. Other optimization methods exist for deriving sharp sensors from Equation 14 including Data-based sharpening (Finlayson et al., 1994b), Tensor-based sharpening (Chong et al., 2007) and Sensor-based sharpening (Finlayson et al., 1994b). Figure 1 gives sharp sensors derived using these last three methods together with the Smith-Pokorny cone fundamentals (Smith & Pokorny, 1975). 
Figure 1
 
Sensitivities of sharp sensors found by the different methods. Data-based sharpening (dashed line), Tensor-based sharpening (dotted line), and sensor-based sharpening (continuous line). Cone fundamentals are plotted in black.
Figure 1
 
Sensitivities of sharp sensors found by the different methods. Data-based sharpening (dashed line), Tensor-based sharpening (dotted line), and sensor-based sharpening (continuous line). Cone fundamentals are plotted in black.
It is clear from this figure that whereas the derived sensors are similar, there is some variation in the results: an observation which raises the question of which sharp sensor set should be used. The question is not immediately easy to answer. Indeed, the optimizations underlying the different sharpening techniques developed hitherto were, in part, formulated so that an optimal solution could be easily found. None of the existing methods finds the sharp sensors which deliver the maximally illuminant-invariant color designators. 
Finding the optimal sharpening transform
Suppose we wish to compute the color designators (Land, 1964) for n reflectances viewed under a D65 illuminant where we map cone responses to sharp counterparts using the 3×3 sharpening matrix T. We calculate the designator for the sth surface:   Dividing Equation 15 by Equation 16 gives the color designator rs the components of which are:  In Equation 17 the color designator has D65 in the superscript. This is because although we seek color designators which are illuminant independent, we will not achieve perfect invariance. Rather, as the illuminant varies, so too will the computed designators. To select the sensors giving the best illuminant independence, we will work with each sensor separately; that is, we will minimize each row of the matrix T individually (we denote each row as Ti). 
Let us define a vector v i D 65 = [ r i s 1 , D 65 , ··· , r i s n , D 65 ]t containing the designators defined in Equation 17 for one of the sensors and a set of surface reflectances under the D65 illuminant, and let v i e = [ r i s 1 , e , , r i s n , e ]t be a vector containing the designators for the same surfaces and the same sensor under another illuminant e. The individual terms for both these vectors are the responses of a single sharp sensor divided by the responses of the light. As the illuminant changes, we expect, for the best sharpening transform, that these vectors of designators will be similar to one another. Assuming m illuminants we seek the transform T which minimizes:   
To find T we shall use the Spherical Sampling technique proposed by Finlayson and Susstrunk (2001). This method treats the sharpening problem combinatorially, defining all possible reasonable sharpening transforms. Without recapitulating the detail, their key insight was that only if two sensors are sufficiently different (by a criterion amount) will this difference impact strongly on color computations. Indeed they argued that for spectral sharpening it suffices only to consider linear combinations of the cones resulting in sensors that are one or more degrees apart. Using this insight, we find there are a discrete number of possible sensors and a discrete number of triplets of sensors. We simply take each of a finite set of sharp sensors and find the red, green, and blue sharp sensor that minimizes Equation 18. The minimization was carried out using the WCS reflectances (a subset of 320 Munsell reflectances) and the same set of illuminants as in Philipona and O'Regan's paper (Chiao, Cronin, & Osorio, 2000; Judd et al., 1964; Romero, Garcia-Beltran, & Hernandez-Andres, 1997). 
The optimal T transform we obtain starting with the Smith-Pokorny cone fundamentals (Smith & Pokorny, 1975) is:  In Figure 2 we plot the corresponding sharp sensors in solid red, green, and blue. 
Figure 2
 
Sensors found using our approach.
Figure 2
 
Sensors found using our approach.
Significantly, we find that Philipona and O'Regan biological reflectance functions (computed using a per surface transformation to virtual sensors) and the color designators (computed with respect to a single global sharpening transform) are strongly correlated (0.9917). Further we calculated the singularity index SPO on the Philipona and O'Regan biological reflectances and the sharp color designators. These too are correlated (0.9251). While not identical, these high correlations provide prima facie evidence that color designators calculated with respect to a single sharpening transform can be used instead of the per-surface biological reflectance functions proposed by Philipona and O'Regan (which are based on a per surface sharpening transform). 
Compact singularity index
The idea of Philipona and O'Regan's singularity index was intuitively clear: they wished the index to be high when one or more of the color designator (or the Philipona and O'Regan biological reflectance) values are close to zero. However, to translate this idea into procedural form required sorting the designator values, calculating inter-band ratios, normalizing these ratios and taking a maximum. Here we suggest a more elegant, compact, singularity measure, which will also turn out to have the advantage of being related to chromaticness. 
Let us begin by writing three terms which measure the relative magnitude of one sensor response relative to the triplet of three responses. Here we use r, g, and b to denote the color designators calculated with respect to our sharp sensitivities (rather than r1, r2, and r3). Further, let us begin by considering singularity in each color channel separately.    By substituting test values into Equation 20 through Equation 22 we see each individual equation implements, correctly, a per channel idea of singularity. As an example, we can see that when r ≈ 0 and g and b are > > 0, then, I2 and I3 will be very large. We simply add these three terms together to define our new Compact Singularity Index:  SC computes a single measure which is large when the rgb designator has one or two values close to 0. Further, the function is symmetric with each of r,g, and b playing the same role. That is, unlike the Philipona and O'Regan definition of singularity (see Equations 11, 12) we need not sort our sensor response or apply a maximum function. 
Compact singularity and chromaticness
While Equation 23 is simple (and as we shall see in the next section also provides a useful lens through which to view WCS color naming detail), it is interesting to examine its structure and to relate it to traditional color concepts. Let us modify our compact singularity index:  Then, we have   
In this form, the numerator intuitively gives us a measure of chromaticness: for achromatic surfaces, where r = g = b, the numerator will be 0 (note that, since we are dealing with designators, illumination effects have been canceled out). In contrast, for any chromatic surface the numerator will be positive, becoming bigger as we move away from the achromatic axis. Significantly, unlike traditional measures of saturation our chromaticness measure is unbounded: as the rgb becomes more and more saturated and the individual channel values go toward zero, so our measure becomes unboundedly large. 
Predictions and results
In one task in the World Color Survey, an average of 24 native speakers of each of 110 unwritten languages were shown a palette of 330 Munsell chips (see Figure 3) and asked to pick out the best (or “focal”) example(s) of the major color terms in their language. Figure 4 shows the histogram and a contour plot of the number of times a given chip was chosen as being such a “focal” color. It is seen that there are four peaks, corresponding to certain precise hues of “red,” “green,” “blue,” and “yellow.” Thus these four hues have a special status across a wide range of cultures. 
Figure 3
 
Munsell chips in the WCS data.
Figure 3
 
Munsell chips in the WCS data.
Figure 4
 
Psychophysical results of the WCS data.
Figure 4
 
Psychophysical results of the WCS data.
A key idea in the Philipona and O'Regan work is that colors that have a high singularity index should correlate with colors frequently characterized as focal. The results of Philipona and O'Regan are shown on the left in Figure 5. From left to right the most kurtotic peaks correspond to “red,” “yellow,” “green,” and “blue” (see Figure 3). At each chip location, the singularity index for each surface is indicated. Clearly, the general trend of the data is quite similar to Figure 4: the singularity index increases and decreases in concert with the WCS histogram, and the peaks are in similar positions. 
Figure 5
 
Comparison between the results obtained by Philipona and O'Regan in their paper (left) and our results using a single global sharpening transform T and the compact singularity index (right). The solid patches of the bottom-plots represent the top 10% of the WCS data.
Figure 5
 
Comparison between the results obtained by Philipona and O'Regan in their paper (left) and our results using a single global sharpening transform T and the compact singularity index (right). The solid patches of the bottom-plots represent the top 10% of the WCS data.
In the present paper we have proposed two extensions of the Philipona and O'Regan work. First, we have calculated color designators with respect to a single sharpening transform (whereas in Philipona and O'Regan's work every surface has its own sharpening transform); second we have used our simplified Compact Singularity Index. The right hand histogram in Figure 5 plots our Compact Singularity Index with respect to the Munsell chips used in the WCS. As in Philipona and O'Regan's, the singularity peaks are in close correspondence with the peaks of the WCS naming histogram. There is a somewhat better correspondence for “blue” compared to the predictions made by Philipona and O'Regan using their singularity index. 
In their paper Philipona and O'Regan also compared their results to psychophysical experiments using “aperture colors.” “Aperture colors” are generated by sending lights with controlled spectral composition directly into the eye rather than by natural viewing of a colored surface. In order to make their approach compatible with the results of such experiments, and in particular in order to find a singularity index for lights instead of for surfaces, Philipona and O'Regan conjectured that the nervous system interprets light from aperture colors as though it corresponded to light coming from colored surfaces illuminated by white light. As referent for white light they chose D65. 
Thus, when illuminated with D65 light, the Munsell chips used in the WCS can be considered to be aperture light sources whose coordinates can be plotted as points in the usual CIE or LMS spaces. For each such light Philipona and O'Regan plotted the singularity of the corresponding Munsell chip, and observed that crests of singularity appeared in the CIE or LMS spaces, which, when extended out to the monochromatic locus, gave predictions for what should be the most singular monochromatic lights. These singular monochromatic lights corresponded very precisely to the so-called “unique hues” observed in psychophysical measurements (see Table 1). The range of variation observed in existing experiments measuring unique hues could also be explained by the width of the areas of singularity defined by Philipona and O'Regan's procedure. 
Table 1
 
Unique hues found in the different experiments and the prediction from our model using a new sensor basis along with the new CSI index. EOS means End Of Spectrum.
Table 1
 
Unique hues found in the different experiments and the prediction from our model using a new sensor basis along with the new CSI index. EOS means End Of Spectrum.
Dataset Subjects Unique Yellow Unique Green
Mean (nm) Range (nm) Mean (nm) Range (nm)
Schefrin 50 577 568–589 509 488–536
Jordan-Mollon 97 512 487–557
Volbrecht 100 522 498–555
Webster (a) 51 576 572–580 544 491–565
Webster (b) 175 580 575–583 540 497–566
Webster (c) 105 576 571–581 539 493–567
Philipona and O'Regan's SI prediction 575 570–580 540 510–560
Our-model reflectances 580 570–585 555 540–565
Our model-sharp sensors 588 585–595 536 515–545
Dataset Subjects Unique Blue Unique Red
Mean (nm) Range (nm) Mean (nm) Range (nm)
Schefrin 50 480 465–495
Jordan-Mollon 97
Volbrecht 100
Webster (a) 51 477 467–485 EOS
Webster (b) 175 479 474–485 605 596–700
Webster (c) 105 472 431–486 EOS
Philipona and O'Regan's SI prediction 465 450–480 625 590-EOS
Our-model reflectances 470 460–480 615 600-EOS
Our model-sharp sensors 464 454–470 607 600–640
Figure 6 shows the results obtained when applying exactly the same method as Philipona and O'Regan to link aperture colors to Munsell chips, with the difference that we use our sharp sensors and our compact singularity index. In Figure 6a we have a 3D plot of the singularity corresponding to 1600 Munsell chips (Joensuu, n.d.), each with the CIE value of the light it reflects when illuminated by D65 daylight. In Figure 6b we are looking at the x-y projection of the figure, and we have circled the four local maxima of the plot. We have connected these maxima to the neutral point, and extrapolated out to the monochromatic locus where we predict the unique hues should be. As seen in Table 1, our predictions are very close to Philipona and O'Regan's, and very close to the empirical data. The range of expected variation of the unique hues can be estimated in our approach by taking the range over which our compact singularity index exceeds some threshold. The range shown in the Table is obtained using a threshold of 15% of the maxima of each different mountain. It also corresponds accurately to the range of unique hues found in the empirical data. However, we should note the existence of the Abney effect: there is some curvature in the lines of perceived hue in the chromaticity diagram. Therefore, our table shows an approximation of the hues. 
Figure 6
 
(a) Plot in x, y space for our new compact singularity index (CSI) for the 1600 Munsell chips. (b) Projection of the compact singularity index and the locus of monochromatic lights.
Figure 6
 
(a) Plot in x, y space for our new compact singularity index (CSI) for the 1600 Munsell chips. (b) Projection of the compact singularity index and the locus of monochromatic lights.
It is worth noting that in our approach, in contrast to Philipona and O'Regan's, there is a more direct method of obtaining unique hue predictions. Instead of extrapolating the LMS values of singular Munsell chips seen under white light out to the monochromatic locus, we can calculate directly the singularity of monochromatic lights. To do this, we again assume that the color designator associated with a light giving a particular sensor response is simply the designator of a surface illuminated by white light that would give that same sensor response. We can now get the designator associated with monochromatic lights by calculating the sharp sensor response to the monochromatic lights, and dividing by the sharp sensor response to white light. For this we have used D65 light. From the designator we can now calculate the singularity, using our compact singularity index. Figure 7 shows this index for all monochromatic lights. We see that there are extrema corresponding closely to the unique hues (see Table 1, last row) and in agreement with the more indirect method of calculation. It is interesting that the singularity in the green region is spread between about 520 and 535 nm. 
Figure 7
 
Unique hues plot computed from the sharp sensors.
Figure 7
 
Unique hues plot computed from the sharp sensors.
Philipona and O'Regan were also able to explain hue cancellation phenomena in their paper. Hue cancellation quantifies the fact that the addition to a light that appears bluish, of a certain amount of light that appears yellowish produces a light that appears neither bluish nor yellowish, and the same for lights that appear reddish or greenish (Jameson & Hurvich, 1955). Note that we are able to explain this data in the same manner Philipona and O'Regan did, by reference to the singularity of Munsell surfaces, transformed into CIE or LMS coordinates by illuminating them with white D65 light. But more interestingly, we are also able to explain the data as an opponent calculation from our spectrally sharp sensors. 
First note that the relative peak sensitivity of our sensors is not fixed in all our previous computations (each sensor was derived independently). We can therefore adjust the peak sensitivities in order to fit the Jameson and Hurvich data. To this end, we have run an optimization to minimize the root mean square error between the Jameson and Hurvich data and our prediction. We do this minimization in two steps. First, we deal with the red-green cancellation by optimizing for αR-βG. The optimal values we obtain are α = 0.56 and β = 0.77. Second, with these values of α and β fixed, we move to the blue-yellow equilibrium, minimizing δ(αR-βG)-(2δ)γB. The -()γ term is defined in this way to have δ regarding the opponency and γ regarding the amplitude of the blue sensor. In this way, δ allows us to adapt the blue-yellow opponency away from the more usual δ = 1. Following this approach we obtain γ = 0.4860. 
In Figure 8 we can see the new sensor amplitudes and the resulting optimal prediction of the Jameson and Hurvich data, which has δ = 0.6477; that is, the blue-yellow opponency is defined as 0.6477(Rc+Gc)-1,2954Bc (already with the amplitude-corrected sensors Rc, Gc, Bc). The two cancellation curves show, on the one hand, the intensity of a monochromatic yellow light that must be added to a bluish light so that the corresponding stimulus is on the locus defining a unique hue different from yellow or blue, and on the other hand the same thing for red and green lights. 
Figure 8
 
Hue cancellation prediction for our index. Left, the normalized sensors found to predict the data. Right, straight lines are our prediction (from an opponent conversion); data symbols represent Jameson and Hurvich data.
Figure 8
 
Hue cancellation prediction for our index. Left, the normalized sensors found to predict the data. Right, straight lines are our prediction (from an opponent conversion); data symbols represent Jameson and Hurvich data.
Our final experiment deals with hue equilibrium. Thanks to our account of the loci of unique hues from singular surface reflection properties, we (as did Philipona and O'Regan) compare our results to the hue equilibrium experiment of Chichilnisky and Wandell (1999). In that experiment, Chichilnisky and Wandell looked for the region of the space where a sensation is neither bluish nor yellowish, and similarly, the region of the space where the sensation is neither reddish nor greenish. Figure 9 provides comparison of Chichilnisky and Wandell's data with the results using Philipona and O'Regan's approach (Figure 9a), and with the results obtained using the Compact Singularity Index following the Philipona and O'Regan reflectance procedure (Figure 9b). In these graphs the data symbols represent Chichilnisky and Wandell's data for subject es while the solid lines represent the predictions using estimations of unique hues shown in Table 1. We can see that our predictions are about as close to the experimental data as those obtained from Philipona and O'Regan's approach. Finally, predictions using unique hues found by sharp sensors are shown in Figure 9c
Figure 9
 
Hue equilibrium prediction. (a) Philipona and O'Regan singularity index. (b) Our compact singularity index with the reflectance procedure. (c) our compact singularity index with the sharp sensors approach. Crosses and triangles are the Chichilniski and Wandell data for their Subject es. Straight lines represent the estimations.
Figure 9
 
Hue equilibrium prediction. (a) Philipona and O'Regan singularity index. (b) Our compact singularity index with the reflectance procedure. (c) our compact singularity index with the sharp sensors approach. Crosses and triangles are the Chichilniski and Wandell data for their Subject es. Straight lines represent the estimations.
Conclusions
Philipona and O'Regan suggested an interesting way of understanding psychophysical data on unique hues and anthropological data on cross-cultural color naming in terms of the linear mapping that describes how reflecting surfaces modify the LMS cone catches of the light that falls on them. 
Their approach defined as “singular,” those surfaces that have the property that they project incoming LMS values into a 1- or 2-dimensional subspace of the 3-dimensional LMS space. They showed that singular surfaces accurately correspond to surfaces in anthropological data that are most frequently considered prototypical in many cultures throughout the world. Singular surfaces are also those that, when illuminated with white light, accurately correspond to hues that appear to be pure or “unique.” Hue cancellation and opponent color matching is also accurately accounted for by Philipona and O'Regan's approach, without any parameter adjustments. 
The main novelty of the present paper is to provide a way of interpreting Philipona and O'Regan's approach in terms of spectral sharpening theory. We show that by replacing the normal LMS sensors by a particular set of sharp sensors, Philipona and O'Regan's notion of singularity can be interpreted as corresponding to lights that activate only one or two, but not three, of the sharp sensors. We show that a single, unique, set of sharp sensors satisfactorily accounts for all the data presented in Philipona and O'Regan's earlier work. In this way, we increase the biological plausibility of the model. 
Importantly, in developing our new theory we define a new singularity index which is more compact than Philipona and O'Regan's and which establishes a clear link to the idea of chromaticness. Singular colors are colors which have high chromaticness on this measure. Our approach provides estimates of color naming, unique hues, hue cancellation, and opponent color matching which are as accurate if not more accurate than Philipona and O'Regan's. 
Acknowledgments
Javier Vazquez-Corral and Maria Vanrell are grateful for support from TIN 2010-21771-C02-1 and Consolider-Ingenio 2010 CSD2007-00018 of Spanish MEC (Ministry of Science). J. K. O'Regan is partially financed by French ANR BINAAHR. Graham Finlayson gratefully acknowledges support from EPSRC grant H022236. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Javier Vazquez-Corral and Graham D. Finlayson. 
Email: jvazquez@cvc.uab.cat; G.Finlayson@uea.ac.uk. 
Address: Computer Science Department, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona and Computer Vision Center, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain; School of Computing Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom. 
References
Berlin B. Kay P . (1969). Basic Color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Burns S. A. Elsner A. E. Pokorny J. Smith V. C . (1984). The Abney effect - Chromaticity coordinates of unique and other constant hues. Vision Research, 24(5),479–489. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cicerone C. M. Krantz D. H. Larimer J . (1975). Opponent-process additivity III. Effect of moderate chromatic adaptation. Vision Research, 15(10),1125–1135. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chiao C. C. Cronin T. W. Osorio D . (2000). Color signals in natural scenes: Characteristics of reflectance spectra and effects of natural illuminants. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 17(2),218–224. [CrossRef]
Chichilnisky E. J. Wandell B. A . (1999). Trichromatic opponent color classification. Vision Research, 39(20),3444–3458. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chong H. Y. Gortler S. J. Zickler T . (2007). The von Kries hypothesis and a basis for color constancy. 2007 Ieee11th International Conference on Computer Vision, 1–6,2143–2150.
Finlayson G. D. Drew M. S. Funt B. V . (1994a). Color constancy - Generalized diagonal transforms suffice. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 11(11),3011–3019. [CrossRef]
Finlayson G. D. Drew M. S. Funt B. V . (1994b). Spectral sharpening - Sensor transformations for improved color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 11(5),1553–1563. [CrossRef]
Finlayson G. D. Susstrunk S . (2001). Spherical sampling and color transformations. Ninth Color Imaging Conference: Color science and engineering systems, technologies, applications,321–325.
Hering E . (1891). Zur Lehre vom Lichtsinne. Sechs Mittheilungen an die Kaiserliche Akademie der Wissenschaften in Wien.
Jameson D. Hurvich L. M . (1955). Some quantitative aspects of an opponent-colors theory I. Chromatic responses and spectral saturation. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 45(7),546–552. [CrossRef]
Joensuu U. o . (n.d.). Colour group. Retrieved from spectral.joensuu.fi.
Judd D. B. Macadam D. L. Wyszecki G. Budde H. W. Condit H. R. Henderson S. T. (1964). Spectral distribution of typical Ddylight as function of correlated color temperature. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 54(8),1031–1036. [CrossRef]
Kay P . (2005). Color categories are not arbitrary. Cross-Cultural Research, 39(1),39–55. [CrossRef]
Kay P. Regier T . (2003). Resolving the question of color naming universals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(15),9085–9089. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kuehni R. G . (2004). Variability in unique hue selection: A surprising phenomenon. Color Research and Application, 29(2),158–162.
Land E . (1964). The retinex. American Scientist, 52,247–264.
Marimont D. H. Wandell B. A . (1992). Linear-models of surface and illuminant Spectra. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 9(11),1905–1913. [CrossRef]
Mollon J. Jordan G . (1997). On the nature of unique hues. John Dalton's Colour Vision-Legacy, 54,391–403.
Parraga C. A. Troscianko T. Tolhurst D. J . (2002). Spatiochromatic properties of natural images and human vision. Current Biology, 12(6),483–487. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Philipona D. L. O'Regan J. K . (2006). Color naming, unique hues, and hue cancellation predicted from singularities in reflection properties. Visual Neuroscience, 23(3–4),331–339. [PubMed]
Romero J. Garcia-Beltran A. Hernandez-Andres J . (1997). Linear bases for representation of natural and artificial illuminants. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 14(5),1007–1014. [CrossRef]
Smith V. C. Pokorny J . (1975). Spectral sensitivity of foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 Nm. Vision Research, 15(2),161–171. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Valberg A . (2001). Unique hues: An old problem for a new generation. Vision Research, 41(13),1645–1657. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Webster M. A. Miyahara E. Malkoc G. Raker V. E . (2000). Variations in normal color vision. II. Unique hues. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 17(9),1545–1555. [CrossRef]
Wuerger S. M. Atkinson P. Cropper S . (2005). The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms. Vision Research, 45(25–26),3210–3223. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wyszecki G. Stiles W. S . (1982). Color science: concepts and methods, quantitative data, and formulae. (2nd ed). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Yendrikhovskij S. N . (2001). Computing color categories from statistics of natural images. Journal of Imaging Science and Technology, 45(5),409–417.
Figure 1
 
Sensitivities of sharp sensors found by the different methods. Data-based sharpening (dashed line), Tensor-based sharpening (dotted line), and sensor-based sharpening (continuous line). Cone fundamentals are plotted in black.
Figure 1
 
Sensitivities of sharp sensors found by the different methods. Data-based sharpening (dashed line), Tensor-based sharpening (dotted line), and sensor-based sharpening (continuous line). Cone fundamentals are plotted in black.
Figure 2
 
Sensors found using our approach.
Figure 2
 
Sensors found using our approach.
Figure 3
 
Munsell chips in the WCS data.
Figure 3
 
Munsell chips in the WCS data.
Figure 4
 
Psychophysical results of the WCS data.
Figure 4
 
Psychophysical results of the WCS data.
Figure 5
 
Comparison between the results obtained by Philipona and O'Regan in their paper (left) and our results using a single global sharpening transform T and the compact singularity index (right). The solid patches of the bottom-plots represent the top 10% of the WCS data.
Figure 5
 
Comparison between the results obtained by Philipona and O'Regan in their paper (left) and our results using a single global sharpening transform T and the compact singularity index (right). The solid patches of the bottom-plots represent the top 10% of the WCS data.
Figure 6
 
(a) Plot in x, y space for our new compact singularity index (CSI) for the 1600 Munsell chips. (b) Projection of the compact singularity index and the locus of monochromatic lights.
Figure 6
 
(a) Plot in x, y space for our new compact singularity index (CSI) for the 1600 Munsell chips. (b) Projection of the compact singularity index and the locus of monochromatic lights.
Figure 7
 
Unique hues plot computed from the sharp sensors.
Figure 7
 
Unique hues plot computed from the sharp sensors.
Figure 8
 
Hue cancellation prediction for our index. Left, the normalized sensors found to predict the data. Right, straight lines are our prediction (from an opponent conversion); data symbols represent Jameson and Hurvich data.
Figure 8
 
Hue cancellation prediction for our index. Left, the normalized sensors found to predict the data. Right, straight lines are our prediction (from an opponent conversion); data symbols represent Jameson and Hurvich data.
Figure 9
 
Hue equilibrium prediction. (a) Philipona and O'Regan singularity index. (b) Our compact singularity index with the reflectance procedure. (c) our compact singularity index with the sharp sensors approach. Crosses and triangles are the Chichilniski and Wandell data for their Subject es. Straight lines represent the estimations.
Figure 9
 
Hue equilibrium prediction. (a) Philipona and O'Regan singularity index. (b) Our compact singularity index with the reflectance procedure. (c) our compact singularity index with the sharp sensors approach. Crosses and triangles are the Chichilniski and Wandell data for their Subject es. Straight lines represent the estimations.
Table 1
 
Unique hues found in the different experiments and the prediction from our model using a new sensor basis along with the new CSI index. EOS means End Of Spectrum.
Table 1
 
Unique hues found in the different experiments and the prediction from our model using a new sensor basis along with the new CSI index. EOS means End Of Spectrum.
Dataset Subjects Unique Yellow Unique Green
Mean (nm) Range (nm) Mean (nm) Range (nm)
Schefrin 50 577 568–589 509 488–536
Jordan-Mollon 97 512 487–557
Volbrecht 100 522 498–555
Webster (a) 51 576 572–580 544 491–565
Webster (b) 175 580 575–583 540 497–566
Webster (c) 105 576 571–581 539 493–567
Philipona and O'Regan's SI prediction 575 570–580 540 510–560
Our-model reflectances 580 570–585 555 540–565
Our model-sharp sensors 588 585–595 536 515–545
Dataset Subjects Unique Blue Unique Red
Mean (nm) Range (nm) Mean (nm) Range (nm)
Schefrin 50 480 465–495
Jordan-Mollon 97
Volbrecht 100
Webster (a) 51 477 467–485 EOS
Webster (b) 175 479 474–485 605 596–700
Webster (c) 105 472 431–486 EOS
Philipona and O'Regan's SI prediction 465 450–480 625 590-EOS
Our-model reflectances 470 460–480 615 600-EOS
Our model-sharp sensors 464 454–470 607 600–640
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×