Given this close association between number and density, does the visual system need independent representations of both, or might they be derived from a common mechanism? This is a contentious issue fuelled by a recent study in which Burr and colleagues (Burr & Ross,
2008b) showed that adaptation to a high numerosity random-dot array reduces the perceived numerosity of a subsequently presented patch of test dots. This led the authors to suggest that numerosity is a primary visual attribute (or “distinct qualia,” Burr & Ross,
2008a) that cannot be reduced to other continuous stimulus dimensions, e.g., density or spatial frequency (Ross & Burr,
2010). This claim has not gone uncontested. Durgin (
2008) showed that, when number and density are uncoupled by changing the size of the adapter patch (the region over which the elements are distributed), adaptation follows the density of the adapter rather than its numerosity (Durgin,
2008), suggesting that density is in fact the adapted dimension (Durgin,
1995). In addition, although not uncontested themselves (Allik, Tuulmets, & Vos,
1991; Burr & Ross,
2008b; Ross & Burr,
2010), several studies have shown that number judgments are sensitive to manipulations of patch size (Tokita & Ishiguchi,
2010), element size (Ginsburg & Nicholls,
1988; Hurewitz, Gelman, & Schnitzer,
2006; Ross,
2003; Sophian,
2007; Tokita & Ishiguchi,
2010), element clustering (Frith & Frith,
1972; Ginsburg,
1978,
1991), and total element coverage (Hurewitz, Gelman, & Schnitzer,
2006; Tokita & Ishiguchi,
2010). Because a “pure” judgment of number should occur regardless of these spatial parameters, such findings are inconsistent with number being extracted as a primary visual attribute independent of other stimulus dimensions.