The results show that when a suddenly appearing stimulus was already represented amodally, observers are less likely to experience motion. As tabulated in
Figure 5, observers were significantly less likely to experience motion when the red flankers were behind the green shape than when they were closer than the green shape. An ANOVA with subject, depth of the red flankers, display variant, and speed of the alternation as factors indicates that the depth of the flankers has a significant effect, F(1,14)=28.7, p<.0001, whereas none of the interactions is significant. There is also a main effect of display variant, with subjects significantly less likely to experience line motion in variant B, F (1,14)=5.7, p=.031. This was expected. The irregular contour of variant B was designed to make the stimulus more amenable to interpretation as green occluding the red. Specifically, in B it is easier to perceive the intersection of the red and green shapes as being intrinsic to the green shape rather than caused by the red occluding the green. A pilot experiment had prompted concern that observers would interpret variant A as the red shape occluding the green even when the stereo disparity specified otherwise, due to the strength of the T-junction as an occlusion cue. Display B was devised to counter this possible tendency towards a ceiling effect.
Presentation rate was also varied in case any particular speed significantly favored one interpretation, but no significant effect obtained. Overall, we have a clear result indicating that amodal completion can determine whether morphing motion is perceived. The sudden appearance of a figure that was already represented, albeit previously invisible, was not experienced as motion.
There were two purposes of including the displays of
Figure 6 in the experiment. First, to determine whether displays even without stereo disparity could nonetheless change from morphing motion to disocclusion on the basis of a viable occlusion interpretation. A second question was whether sudden onsets could be interpreted as disocclusion even without an amodal representation prompted by the static display. This possibility arose because in the displays of
Figure 6A and
Figure 6E, the individual frames were not sufficient to induce the perception of occlusion.
The first frame of 6A is typically perceived as a red rectangle adjacent to a green rectangle, and in 6E the blue rectangle is perceived as flanked by a coplanar reddish shape and orange shape. However, the second frame of these displays suggests that, in the case of 6A, the red shape was but the visible portion of a larger bar, and in the case of 6E, that the orange and reddish shapes were in fact part of one shape behind a blue rectangle. This constitutes a dynamic cue to occlusion, as it requires the comparison of two frames. The question was whether this dynamic occlusion cue will reduce, compared to control displays, the perception of morphing motion of the red rectangle in 6A and of the orange/red shapes in 6E.
Indeed the data indicate that the dynamic occlusion cue was effective. The probability of perceiving morphing motion was much lower in the display of
Figure 6A than in the control displays of
Figure 6B (t(28)=−4.91, p<.0001) and 6C (t (28)=−3.35, p=.0012). This result was predicted, as the display of
Figure 6A is more consistent with disocclusion of the extension of the red than are displays 6B and 6C. The cue to disocclusion is the coincident disappearance of the green shape when the extension of the red appeared. Still, a critic might attribute the difference in number of subjects reporting motion to some general disruption of motion signals caused by the transient of the green shape’s disappearance. The result with control display 6D counters that criticism. In display 6D the green rectangle disappeared just as it did in 6A, but the display in 6D was less consistent with the red being amodally completed behind and indeed motion was more likely to be perceived (t(28)=−1.89, p=.035).
Comparison of display E with control display F further supports the hypothesis that a motionless dynamic pictorial occlusion cue can cause the perception of disocclusion instead of morphing motion. When the first frame of display E is viewed alone, typically three aligned shapes are perceived rather than any occluding relationships. This is consistent with an interpretation of morphing motion when the next frame appears — the orange shapes morph together, closing like curtains over the blue rectangle. However, only 47% of observers perceived motion in this display. In this experiment observers did not report the nature of the occlusion perceived, so we cannot be sure of their interpretation, but in an unpublished experiment all those who reported motion described it as the curtain-closing percept. In the present experiment, instead of perceiving motion in display E, most reported that the blue shape appeared to dematerialize with the appearance of the second frame, revealing the center of the longer shape that previously had been occluded. Hence disocclusion was perceived despite the lack of any clue to the existence of the hidden material in the first frame. This suggests that the dynamic covering and uncovering itself can sometimes result in a disocclusion percept, effectively vetoing a morphing motion percept.
Once again, in display E, approximately half of observers perceived dematerialization of the blue, revealing an orange shape behind. None of the observers ever reported this in the case of display F, even though the center strip of display F was identical to display E. Instead, in display F observers reported morphing motion (100% vs. 47% of display E, t(28)=−4.0, p=.0002). The addition of the contiguous blue context to E, creating display F, resulted in interpretation of blue as the background. With the orange pattern seen in front, then, appearance of the orange could not be attributed to disocclusion and thus morphing motion of the orange was perceived.
Together these displays show that the percept of occlusion vs. dematerialization can occur even when no single frame yields amodal completion. Interestingly, the dynamic occlusion cues of these displays are dynamic but do not involve motion, despite the emphasis in the literature on motion as the dynamic occlusion cue.
A final point is that none of the observers perceived the blue shape of display 6E to transform or morph into the visible material of the second display. When one stimulus replaces another, how does the system decide whether to consider it a transformation of the old object instead of a new object? In the present case, the decision clearly has something to do with whether an occlusion relationship is plausible, but this question remains mostly unexplored.
Although in display F the prediction that morphing would be perceived was fulfilled without exception, most of the manipulations resulted in more modest increases in the likelihood of the predicted outcome. One reason is that the cues manipulated in the displays did not always result in a change in the perceived depth. For example, in the case of the displays in which stereo disparity was manipulated (
Figure 5), a preliminary experiment showed that despite stereodisparity’s reputation as a potent depth cue, for many naïve subjects it did not overcome the interpretation they favored when the stimulus had no stereodisparity. Such dominance by non-stereo cues is not unprecedented (for a review, see
Howard & Rogers 2002).
The other displays were also sometimes seen in ways unanticipated by the author. Furthermore, it seemed that attention could be used to perceive motion where at first glance motion was not perceived. This is consistent with previous results that, at least with some stimuli, attention can determine the motion perceived (
Verstraten, Cavanagh, & Labianca, 2000;
Cavanagh 1992). Conflicts between motion energy and ecological cues in some displays probably were another contributor to the variability of the results. Because of this variability, and especially because attention allows observer expectations to influence the results, collecting data from naïve subjects is quite important. In some previous reports in the literature, the methods were unfortunately not described, making the number of naïve observers used unknown.