The experimenter selects
N stimuli,
S1, …,
N and prepares a list of quadruples, (
i, j; k, l). It is convenient to avoid repetitions in the list of indices. If, for example, the observer is presented with the quadruple (
1,2;1,3) then he can potentially note that the ‘right’ answer is signaled by the ordering of the second stimulus in each interval. He can then base his judgment solely on the basis of an ordering of the stimuli. The total possible number of stimuli with distinct indices 1≤
i <
j <
k <
l ≤
N is

, which can be written out as,
Some values are tabulated for small values of
N in
Table 1. Given any quadruple, (
i,j,k,l), with 1 ≤
i <
j <
k <
l ≤
N, it may be presented to the subject in eight different ways. For example, in the color stimuli in
Figure 2, we could present (
i,j) above and (
k,l below or vice versa. Wherever we chose to present (
i,j), we can present
Si on the left and
Skitalic; on the right, or vice versa. Similarly for (
k,l). Since the ordering of the stimuli (
i <
j) should be obvious to the observer, there is little point in bothering to randomize left-right and we do not. We do, however, randomize the locations of (
i, j) and (
k, l), effectively flipping a coin to decide which one goes above or below, or, for temporal forced-choice, which one goes first or second.
For ten stimuli, then, one first pass through all possible intervals in random order requires 210 judgments (
Table 1). Assuming that these forced-choice judgments take no more than a 5 seconds each, it is possible to go through 210 judgments in about 20 minutes or less.
For 20 or more stimuli, the total number of possible trials becomes too large to contemplate. However, it is still possible to carry out difference scaling and parameter estimation using only a fraction of the possible trials. In a later section, we investigate how many trials are actually needed to establish a difference scale for N stimuli for values of N larger than 10.
Of course, we can also repeat the series of 210 trials for 10 stimuli as many times as we like. In the next section we examine how the bias and reliability of the estimates vary with the number of trials.