An ideal measurement tool for learning should not produce learning. The MBT method fulfils this requirement, showing no significant learning in contrast discrimination (
Experiment 2 and
3). This could be attributed to reduced efficiency of selection processes involved in learning when uncertainty regarding stimulus parameters exists (compare the results of
Experiment 6a and
6b). Surprisingly, the added uncertainty did not affect discrimination thresholds before learning, as CD thresholds did not differ significantly between the blocked and the MBT methods in pre-learned observers (as in
Experiment 2, but not after learning as in
Experiment 5). This latter result was found to depend on the testing order, showing increased thresholds with the MBT method when tested before the blocked method (
Experiment 3). We took this result (
Figure 6) as evidence for a fast-learning process that had taken place during practice using the blocked method. This effect also saturated very quickly (within the first block/session), because no significant learning effect was seen in the data obtained with the blocked method (data presented in
Figure 2,
Figure 5b, and
Figure 6b). The learning effect could only be observed when the observer moved from the high-uncertainty conditions (MBT) to the no-uncertainty condition (blocked), and back to the MBT method. Note that this fast-learning effect, though obtained with the blocked method, was transferred to the MBT method, suggesting that it involved modification of low-level networks. As shown in
Figure 6, on the average, this learning effect is not too strong and is significant only for the highest base contrast (0.5). The literature indicates that learning processes due to improved selection are relatively fast, in particular when a “difficult” task is preceded by an easy one (Ahissar & Hochstein,
1997; Liu & Weinshall,
2000; Rubin, Nakayama, & Shapley,
1997). Thus, the effect might reflect improved selection among multiple-contrast filters (Geisler & Albrecht,
1997) during practice with blocked contrasts. On this account, the observers monitor selected contrast channels. It is possible that unpracticed observers do not use their knowledge about base contrast to weigh the contrast channels to achieve better performance, but with repeated practice, they learn to select and better weight the appropriate channels to produce a response (Lu & Dosher,
2004). Such a selection can be implemented by association between the different processing units, within or between the different levels of processing, enabling the retainment of the decision scheme for future use. Note that a strong interaction between top-down (i.e., task demands) and bottom-up modulation is involved in perceptual learning (Ahissar & Hochstein,
1993; Meinhardt,
2002; Shiu & Pashler,
1992). Thus, decision-driven perceptual learning may induce changes at the sensory level, possibly by selecting the behaviorally relevant neuronal population as the target for the learning process. This selection process, often termed “gating of learning,” is an essential factor in sensory development during the critical period (Held & Hein,
1963), and some of the neuronal correlates were recently identified (Fregnac, Shulz, Thorpe, & Bienenstock,
1988). The results of
Experiment 6 are in agreement with this concept. We found a difference in the magnitude of the learning effect, depending on whether stimuli were mixed (MBT) or blocked in a session, with the blocked method producing a stronger learning effect (
Figure 12 vs.
Figure 11). It is possible that the gating process fails when stimulus properties are not well specified.