We found that the luminance-color correlation had an influence on the L-cone difference scores in all configurations. This finding is consistent with that of Golz and MacLeod (
2002), who used equivalent experimental conditions. However, our results suggest that Golz and MacLeod (
2002) were incorrect in their implicit assumptions that the visual system uses the correlation between luminance and color in the whole scene to derive the chromaticity of the illuminant. For the luminance-color correlation to provide reliable data for estimating the chromaticity of the illuminant (and, thereby, to separate surface properties from those of the illumination), it is crucial that not just a small part of the visual field is considered, because otherwise the colors of objects which happen to be within the relevant part (e.g., next to the object of interest) will dominate the perceived color (Brenner & Cornelissen,
1991).
We found that extending the color-luminance correlation beyond 1 deg of the test disk had little effect on color appearance. This spatial property is consistent with the spatial properties of chromatic induction (Walraven,
1973; Tiplitz-Blackwell & Buchsbaum,
1988; Brenner & Cornelissen,
1991). This raises the possibility that the present findings and those of Golz and MacLeod (
2002) are the result of an interaction between color and luminance when the border contrast is determined. Asymmetries between the chromatic influences of brighter and darker back-ground surfaces have been found before (e.g., Delahunt & Brainard,
2000; Bauml,
2001; Delahunt & Brainard,
2004). In our case, we always have both brighter and darker squares next to the target. However, if the squares that have a higher luminance have a stronger influence on the perceived color, and the effects of all the surrounding squares are additive (Brenner, Cornelissen, & Nuboer,
1989), the summed effect will depend on which color was brighter. Such an asymmetry could explain our data. Moreover, it provides a way to use the ideas underlying Goltz and MacLeod’s proposal for a modest contribution to color constancy without assuming that the illumination is uniform (which it seldom is in daily life).
The overall pattern of the difference scores for the two color-balancing methods was the same. This is not very surprising considering that the difference was extremely small, but it ensures us that the influence that we found is not just a consequence of having equated the fields at the wrong stage of processing. At least, our findings hold whether one equates the fields at the cone (matched sum balancing method) or at the color-opponent (matched ratio balancing method) stages of processing.
In conclusion, while we agree with Golz and Mac-Leod (
2002) that there is a bias in chromatic induction away from the color of bright surfaces, we show that this bias is not used, as they implicitly suggest, to estimate the chromaticity of the illuminant from the correlation between luminance and chromaticity within the whole scene.