In the present study, we examine the relation between the content of typical natural scenes and behavioral performance with respect to the dimension of orientation. As with the ecological relationships concerning amplitude spectrum slope discussed above, orientation also has often been discussed from an ecological standpoint. Such a standpoint links three lines of research. First, it has been shown that human visual sensitivity and acuity is typically better at horizontal and vertical orientations than at oblique orientations (Campbell, Kulikowski, & Levinson
1966; Mitchell, Freeman, & Westheimer,
1967), which has been termed the
oblique effect (Appelle,
1972) or specifically the
class 1 oblique effect (Essock,
1980; Essock, Krebs, & Prather,
1992). Secondly, a neurophysiological oblique effect has been documented in humans with visual evoked potentials (Maffei & Campbell,
1970; Zemon, Gutowski & Horton,
1983) as well as with functional magnetic resonance imaging (Furmanski & Engel,
2000) where horizontal and vertical orientations are favored over oblique orientations in humans. Such a bias has also been demonstrated in other animals with respect to numbers of cortical cells devoted to the different orientations via single unit recordings (e.g., Mansfield,
1974; Mansfield & Ronner,
1978; Kennedy, Martin, Orban, & Whitteridge,
1985; De Valois, Yund, & Hepler,
1982; Li, Peterson, & Freeman,
2003), with more cells sampled that prefer horizontal and vertical orientations relative to the oblique orientations. Consistent with a greater number of neurons at these orientations, larger cortical regions are observed at horizontal and vertical orientations relative to oblique orientations (Chapman, Stryker, & Bonhoeffer,
1996; Chapman, & Bonhoeffer,
1998; Coppola, White, Fitzpatrick, & Purves,
1998; Yu & Shou,
2000). The third related line of research findings, the ecological component, arises when one considers the multiple reports where the content of natural scenes (and scenes with
carpentered content) is reported to be least prevalent at oblique orientations and most prevalent at horizontal and vertical orientations (Switkes, Mayer, & Sloan,
1978; Baddeley & Hancock,
1991; Hancock, Baddeley, & Smith,
1992; Coppola, Purves, McCoy, & Purves,
1998; Keil & Cristóbal,
2000). Specifically, these three types of anisotropy have led to the oft-cited dogma that it would be advantageous for animals to have best vision at those orientations that are most prevalent in the environment, and that, through either ontologic (Annis & Frost,
1973) or phylogenetic (Timney & Muir,
1976) means, this anisotropy develops to match the most prevalent content in the anisotropic world.