In our 2IFC stereomotion speed discrimination task, the speed of a test stimulus was manipulated during testing to match the speed of the standard, which remained constant. Test stimuli in all conditions featured identical speeds of monocular motion in each eye, moving in opposite directions, and hence moved directly away from the binoculus along the mid-line (see stimulus D,
Figure 8). This stimulus had no lateral motion, because the average of left and right eye velocities was zero.
Standard stimuli featured several possible trajectories of receding stereomotion in separate conditions, all of which lay on the horizontal meridian (i.e., contained no vertical motion). These trajectories were created in two orthogonal manipulations by varying (a) the relative speeds and (b) the relative directions of the left and right eye monocular image motion (see
Figure 8). First, we created stimuli with either equal monocular speeds or with a ratio of 2:1 (or 1:2). When speeds were equal in both eyes, standard stimuli, like their test counterparts, receded directly away from the binoculus, along the mid-line (referred to as condition D). When monocular image speed was faster in the left compared to the right eye, the stimulus trajectory was angled obliquely away from the right eye (referred to as condition R). Conversely, with a faster speed in the right eye, the stimulus trajectory was angled away from the left eye (referred to as condition L).
In a second manipulation, we created two types of obliquely moving stimuli. One featured monocular motions in opposite directions with an extrapolated trajectory that intercepted the interocular axis in between the two eyes. The other featured monocular motions in the same direction with an extrapolated trajectory that did not. These two classes of trajectory will be referred to as “hitting the head” (or Hit) and “missing the head” (or Miss), respectively.
The details of
Experiment 3 differed from those of
Experiment 1 only in the following respects. All stimuli moved with a relative disparity pedestal of zero (i.e., passed through the plane of the background dots at the midpoint of its motion). They simulated a real-world trajectory inclined at + 0.25° for Hit stimuli, or + 2.23° for Miss stimuli, with respect to the midline.
For the small trajectory angles used here, the difference between vz and the total 3D speed, or v, is negligible (<0.0001% for Hit stimuli; <0.1% for Miss stimuli). We kept the sum of the (unsigned) monocular speeds for all standard stimuli identical in all conditions. This meant that the amplitude of the (signed) velocity difference between the monocular motions in the two eyes (vR – vl), and hence the speeds of motion in depth (vz) were different for Hit and Miss conditions. For Hit stimuli, the standard speed was 0.622 deg/s (equivalent to a vz value of 1.04 m/s), and for Miss conditions, 0.207 deg/s (equivalent to a vz value of 0.347 m/s).
The staircase routine determined the speed of the test stimulus from a set of 9 possible values. For Hit stimuli, possible speeds of motion experienced by each eye simultaneously were 0.178, 0.207, 0.249, 0.276, 0.311, 0.355, 0.373, 0.414, and 0.466 deg/s toward the nasal side, which correspond to interocular velocity differences of 0.355, 0.414, 0.497, 0.553, 0.622, 0.710, 0.746, 0.829, and 0.932 deg/s (or real-world vz values between 0.63 and 1.56 m/s). For Miss stimuli, monocular speeds were 0.083, 0.104, 0.138, 0.207, 0.311, 0.373, 0.414, 0.466, and 0.533 corresponding to velocity differences of 0.166, 0.207, 0.276, 0.414, 0.622, 0.710, 0.746, 0.829, and 0.932 deg/s (or real-world vz values between 0.28 and 1.56 m/s). The Michelson contrast of stimulus patterns was reduced to 20%, to ensure that both monocular images featured perceptually smooth motion at the slower stimulus speeds needed here. In all trials, stimuli were visible for 600 ms, with an inter-stimulus interval of 600 ms.
The same four subjects who participated in
Experiment 2 contributed data. Except for one subject (the author LS), all participants were naïve as to the purpose of the experiment.
Subjects completed three sessions of testing, with each session consisting of 8 blocks (4 using Hit stimuli and 4 using Miss in random order), and each block lasting approximately 5 min. Each block contained 3 randomly interleaved staircases (12 reversals each), one for each of the 3 trajectory conditions (D, L and R). We sequentially presented the standard and test stimuli in a randomized order. Subjects were asked to indicate whether the first or second stimulus receded in depth more rapidly, regardless of the degree of lateral translation.
Data were analyzed for each subject individually. Responses from trials in a given condition were combined across the 4 blocks within a session and a cumulative Gaussian curve was fitted to the data by probit analysis (Finney,
1971). Both the mean (PSE) and the
SD of the underlying Gaussian distribution were free parameters in the curve fitting routine. PSEs and thresholds were then averaged across sessions and their associated
SEs calculated. Statistical significance was assessed using ANOVAs, with specific comparisons performed using Bonferroni corrected
t tests.