Free
Research Article  |   April 2005
A cellular and molecular model of response kinetics and adaptation in primate cones and horizontal cells
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision April 2005, Vol.5, 5. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/5.4.5
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Hans van Hateren; A cellular and molecular model of response kinetics and adaptation in primate cones and horizontal cells. Journal of Vision 2005;5(4):5. https://doi.org/10.1167/5.4.5.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

A model for the sensitivity regulation in the primate outer retina is developed and validated using horizontal cell measurements from the literature. The main conclusion is that the phototransduction of the cones is the key factor regulating sensitivity. The model consists of a nonlinearity cascaded with three feedback control loops. The nonlinearity is caused by the hydrolysis of cGMP by activated phosphodiesterase. The first feedback loop is divisive, with calcium regulating the photocurrent in the cone outer segment. The second feedback loop is also divisive, with voltage-sensitive channels regulating the membrane voltage of the cone inner segment. The final feedback loop is subtractive, where the membrane voltage of the horizontal cell is subtracted from that of the cone before the cone drives the horizontal and bipolar cells. The model describes adequately the major characteristics of the horizontal cell responses to wide field, spectrally white stimuli. In particular, it shows (1) sensitivity and bandwidth control as a function of background intensity; (2) the major nonlinearities observed in the horizontal cells; and (3) the transition from linear responses toward contrast constancy (Weber’s law) for background illuminances ranging from 1–1000 td.

Introduction
The light intensity of visual stimuli varies enormously in natural environments. Visual systems generally cope with these variations by regulating sensitivity to light at an early stage of visual processing. In the visual system of the macaque, Smith, Pokorny, Lee, and Dacey (2001) showed that such a regulation could be measured at the level of the horizontal cells in the outer retina. These cells are directly postsynaptic to the cones. They are thought to provide feedback to the cone pedicles, and thus regulate the temporal, spatial, and spectral properties of the output of the cones to the bipolar cells. 
A fully adequate model of sensitivity regulation in the outer retina is lacking, whereas it is clearly important for modeling visual processing upstream in the retina and beyond. It should necessarily form the first stage of any model of the visual system that could function well in natural, outdoor-lighting conditions. The present study, therefore, aims to develop such a model, using the extensive set of horizontal cell measurements recently published (Lee, Dacey, Smith, & Pokorny 2003; Smith et al., 2001). It will be shown that the sensitivity regulation observed in horizontal cells with wide field, spectrally white stimuli, is fully consistent with the regulation expected from the known processes of the phototransduction machinery of the cones. 
Model
Both phototransduction and the neural circuit of cone and horizontal cell have been extensively investigated, and much is known about the physiological processes that determine their dynamics (for reviews of phototransduction, see Arshavsky, Lamb, & Pugh 2002; Burns & Baylor, 2001; Fain, Matthews, Cornwall, & Koutalos 2001; Pugh & Lamb, 2000; for a review of horizontal cells, see Kamermans & Spekreijse, 1999). Below I will represent a series of these processes, first as differential equations describing their dynamics, and subsequently as a system model consisting entirely of first-order low-pass filters and static nonlinearities. Because many readers may not be fully familiar with all of these fields, I will present the key topics in a relatively self-contained manner. The variables in the model have different physical dimensions, ranging from trolands through ion currents and concentrations to electrical currents and voltages. To keep the equations as concise as possible, I will not explicitly include dimensional conversion constants, and I will allow gains that are not dimensionless. Wherever the dimensions in an equation appear not to match, the reader should assume an implicit conversion constant of unit dimension. 
Low-pass filters and static nonlinearities
In the sections below I will show that the (coupled) differential equations describing the system can be translated into a system model with only two types of components: first-order low-pass filters and static nonlinearities. The fact that the behavior of these components is straightforward and well understood greatly simplifies understanding and analysis of the system dynamics. I will therefore first introduce these basic components. 
The standard equation describing a low-pass filter with time constant τy, transforming an input signal x(t) into an output signal y(t), is  
(1)
with the dot denoting time differentiation:  
(2)
Figure 1A shows how this filter is depicted in the models below. The solution of Equation 1 is  
(3)
which is the convolution of the input function x(t) with the impulse response of the filter,  
(4)
The low-frequency (DC-) gain of this filter equals 1, thus low frequencies are not changed by the filter. 
Figure 1
 
A. Representation of a first-order low-pass filter transforming x(t) into y(t), with time constant τy and DC-gain 1. B. Astatic nonlinear function f(x) transforms x into y for each t.
Figure 1
 
A. Representation of a first-order low-pass filter transforming x(t) into y(t), with time constant τy and DC-gain 1. B. Astatic nonlinear function f(x) transforms x into y for each t.
An alternative form of a low-pass filter that often occurs when dealing with chemical reactions and membrane processes is given by a variant of Equation 1:  
(5)
This equation is associated with an impulse response  
(6)
The low-frequency gain now equals τy. An example of such a process, where gain and time constant are covarying, is the voltage response to a current injected into a cell: When the membrane resistance is lowered, both the membrane time constant and the gain (voltage response to a given current) decrease. Below, such processes are described as the cascade of a separate gain τy and a standard low-pass filter as in Figure 1A
The second component used in the models below is the static (memory-less) nonlinearity depicted in Figure 1B: It gives, for each point in time, the output y(t) as a function f(x(t)) of the input x(t). The function f(·) itself is not a function of time, only of the present value of its input x. A special case is the function f(x) = gx, with g a constant describing a (linear) gain. 
Outer segment: Phosphodiesterase activation and cGMP hydrolysis
For the equations describing the phototransduction cascade I will keep the notation as short as possible by mostly using single letter symbols for the variables and constants involved. Furthermore, I will keep the number of variables, such as scaling constants, to a bare minimum, and include only those that affect the calculations when fitting to the measurements on horizontal cells. Cascaded scaling factors or gains will therefore be merged into a single factor or gain wherever possible. Many of the equations are based on modeling work by Calvert, Govardovskii, Arshavsky, and Makino (2002), Detwiler, Ramanathan, Sengupta, and Shraiman (2000), Hamer (2000), Koutalos and Yau (1996), Pugh and Lamb (2000), Schnapf, Nunn, Meister, and Baylor (1990), and in particular Nikonov, Engheta, and Pugh (1998), and Nikonov, Lamb, and Pugh (2000). These studies also give extensive references to the original experimental studies that have led to the understanding of the dynamics of the various parts of the system. 
Figure 2A shows on the left the reaction scheme leading from the absorption of light (I) by the visual pigment (R) via a G-protein (G) to the production of activated phosphodiesterase (E*). In the first step, the visual pigment absorbs light, and is converted into an active form (R*). The active form is removed with a rate constant kR. It is assumed here that the light intensity is sufficiently low such that the concentration of R is not significantly affected (new R is produced fast enough, and there is no significant bleaching of the pigment). The reaction is then described by  
(7)
with I the retinal illuminance in trolands, and AR a scaling constant. This can be rewritten as  
(8)
which can be recognized as a low-pass filter with time constant τR and gain τRAR
Figure 2
 
A. Light I produces excited visual pigment R*, which produces excited G-protein G*, activating phosphodiesterase (PDE). The system diagram to the right describes only the associated low-pass filtering, whereas the gain of the reactions is incorporated into the constant kβ shown in B. B. Production of X (cGMP) under conditions of clamped calcium (i.e., fixed α). The differential equation describing production and removal of cGMPcan be interpreted as shown in the system diagram on the right: a static nonlinearity 1/β followed by low-pass filtering with a time constant τX = 1/β.
Figure 2
 
A. Light I produces excited visual pigment R*, which produces excited G-protein G*, activating phosphodiesterase (PDE). The system diagram to the right describes only the associated low-pass filtering, whereas the gain of the reactions is incorporated into the constant kβ shown in B. B. Production of X (cGMP) under conditions of clamped calcium (i.e., fixed α). The differential equation describing production and removal of cGMPcan be interpreted as shown in the system diagram on the right: a static nonlinearity 1/β followed by low-pass filtering with a time constant τX = 1/β.
In the second step, R* activates a G-protein into its active form, G*, which subsequently forms an active complex E* with phosphodiesterase (PDE). Although the latter reaction can be modeled as a separate low-pass filter, its time constant is assumed to be so short that it can be neglected (Lamb & Pugh, 1992). It is again assumed that light intensities are such that neither G nor PDE is significantly reduced. Therefore, the production of E* is described by  
(9)
with kE the rate of E* inactivation. This is also a low-pass filter, with time constant τE and gain τEAE. For simplicity, this gain and the one in Equation 8 are merged into the gain, kβ, describing the activity of E* (see below). Equations 8 and 9 can therefore be represented by the system model on the right side of Figure 2A
Figure 2B shows on the left the reaction scheme where E* reduces the concentration of cGMP (symbolized by X below), which is itself produced at a rate α. The activity of PDE, β, is described by a constant dark activity (here cβ, often called βdark in the literature) plus a term depending on the amount of activated PDE:  
(10)
with kβ a constant. The concentration of cGMP is then described by  
(11)
At first sight, this looks like a complicated nonlinear equation, because the input, β, is multiplied by the output, X. However, it is possible to reformulate this equation as a static nonlinearity followed by a first-order low-pass filter with variable time constant. This can be seen be rewriting Equation 11 as  
(12)
In this equation, the input 1/β is low-pass filtered into an output X, with a time constant τX depending on the input, and a gain α. Assuming for the moment that the gain α is constant (it is in fact under calcium control; see below), Equations 10 and 12 can be represented by the system model on the right side of Figure 2B. The importance of 1/β for setting the sensitivity and time constant of the system has been discussed for rods (see Nikonov et al., 2000). 
Outer segment: The calcium feedback loop
Figure 3 shows on the left the reaction scheme of the calcium feedback loop. An increase in cGMP concentration (X) will open more cyclic nucleotide-gated (CNG) ion channels in the plasma membrane of the outer segment, resulting in an inward current consisting partly of Ca2+. The resulting increase in calcium concentration is counter-acted by a Na+/Ca2+-K+ exchanger, and possibly by calcium buffering. The increased calcium concentration reduces the rate with which guanylate cyclase (GC) synthesizes cGMP, and thus counteracts the initial rise of cGMP. In a secondary loop, more important in cones than in rods (Rebrik & Korenbrot, 2004), Ca2+ decreases the sensitivity of the CNG channels to cGMP. 
Figure 3
 
In the calcium control loop, an increase in cGMP (X) opens more membrane channels, increasing the photocurrent Ios and the calcium influx. The increase in calcium reduces, via guanylate cyclase (GC), the production of cGMP, counteracting the initial rise. In a secondary negative feedback loop, calcium reduces the effectiveness of the membrane channels (dashed lines).
Figure 3
 
In the calcium control loop, an increase in cGMP (X) opens more membrane channels, increasing the photocurrent Ios and the calcium influx. The increase in calcium reduces, via guanylate cyclase (GC), the production of cGMP, counteracting the initial rise. In a secondary negative feedback loop, calcium reduces the effectiveness of the membrane channels (dashed lines).
The Ca2+ concentration, called C here, is described by  
(13)
where η is a scaling constant describing which proportion of the total photocurrent, Ios, is carried by calcium, and kC is the removal rate of calcium, presumably due to the exchanger. Thus C is a low-pass filtered version of the photo-current, with time constant τC and gain τcν. For the calculations, this gain will be merged into the scaling constant aC determining α, as described below. 
The reaction scheme contains two nonlinearities due to cooperativity. The opening of the CNG channels depends on the cGMP concentration as  
(14)
with Ios the photocurrent into the outer segment, and nX typically 3 (Koutalos & Yau, 1996). Scaling is incorporated into the scaling constant aC below, and into the scaling constant ais determining the voltage response of the inner segment (see next section). The activity with which GC produces cGMP is, for the values of C relevant for the fits, described by  
(15)
with aC a scaling constant, and nC typically taken to be 2 (Koutalos & Yau, 1996). The numerator of Equation 15 is scaled to 1 without loss of generality, because all scaling needed for the model fits is incorporated into the scaling constants aC and ais
Equations 1215 are represented by the system model on the right side of Figure 3. To emphasize that it is a negative feedback loop and to enable an easy comparison with existing models, it is drawn as a divisive gain control: The input Q(=1/β) is divided by 1/α, which is itself an expansive function of the calcium concentration, and therefore provides strong negative feedback. The broken line in the Figure represents the reduction of the channel sensitivity caused by calcium: either through the calcium concentration C or, possibly more accurately, through the calcium current if the interactions are restricted to local domains. To keep the model as simple as possible, I am not modeling this interaction in detail. Instead, I assume it accounts for the fact that the standard value nX = 3 cannot produce acceptable fits of the model to the measurements. It produces major deficiencies in the sensitivities as a function of contrast and mean intensity. Leaving nX as a free parameter in the fits leads to much lower values, typically nX = 1. This low value of nX may be the result of the direct calcium feedback onto the CNG channels: Channel openings caused by an increase in X are counteracted by the subsequent increase in calcium influx. It is well known in engineering that negative feedback loops can apparently linearize nonlinearities in the forward path. For example, a fast divisive feedback loop with a squaring operation in the feedback path will have output = input/output2, or output = input1/3, thus converting an actual nX = 3 into an apparent nX = 1. I will assume the value nX = 1 below. 
Similarly, I found that the fits, although not unreasonable for values nC = 2 or 3, were significantly better for nC ≈ 4. An apparent value of nC = 4 was recently reported and discussed for rods (Burns, Mendez, Chen, & Baylor, 2002). I will assume nC = 4 below. 
Inner segment and cone pedicle
The inner segment contains a range of ion channels, partly voltage sensitive, shaping the response (Yagi & MacLeish, 1994). To model this without introducing many (poorly known) parameters, I will follow here the approach of Detwiler, Hodgkin, and McNaughton (1980). It is assumed that the overall steady-state conductance of the membrane, gis, is given by a (nonlinear) function of the receptor potential Vis:  
(16)
where Vis is defined relative to the resting potential (i.e., the potential when Ios = 0). The voltage response to an abrupt change in Ios is given by the instantaneous conductance of the membrane, gi, thus  
(17)
Finally, it is assumed that the instantaneous conductance approaches the steady-state conductance according to first-order kinetics:  
(18)
The precise form of gis(Vis) is not known for primate cones, but I found that those aspects of horizontal cell responses that are presumably generated at the inner segment (in particular, response sagging, rebounds after pulses and steps, and reduced sensitivity at low frequencies) are well modeled by assuming  
(19)
where ais is a scaling constant, and γ is a constant that is approximately 0.7 according to the fits below. 
The change of Vis in response to a change in Ios is in reality not as instantaneous as suggested by Equation 17, because the membrane capacitance has to be charged. This can be represented by an additional low-pass filter with time constant τm, assumed to be approximately constant. The gain of this filter is merged into the scaling constant ais. The above equations lead to the system model shown in Figure 4. Although the signal transfer from the cone inner segment to the cone pedicle may produce some additional filtering (Hsu, Tsukamoto, Smith, & Sterling, 1998), it is assumed here that the signal arriving at the cone pedicle is that of the inner segment. An alternative interpretation is that any additional filtering can be thought to be incorporated into the parameters γ, ais, τis, and τm used here for the model of the inner segment. 
Figure 4
 
Model of the filtering properties of the inner segment.
Figure 4
 
Model of the filtering properties of the inner segment.
The cone-horizontal cell feedback loop
For the feedback from the horizontal cell to the cone pedicle, I assume the subtractive scheme shown in Figure 5A. This is consistent with recent proposals for the feedback mechanism (Kamermans, Fahrenfort, Schultz, Janssen-Bienhold, Sjoerdsma, et al., 2001), and also with the spatially local character of the gain control (see Discussion). The driving force Vs for transmitter release It is determined by the difference of the voltage of the inner segment, Vis, and the voltage Vh of the horizontal cell multi-plied by a gain gh. Thus  
(20)
In the calculations below, gh is fixed to 1, because it can be merged with the forward gain gs. Going around the loop there are three low-pass filters, which together with the gain gs determine the characteristics of the resonant oscillations observed in horizontal cells. The minimum number of low-pass filters required to obtain oscillations is two, but I found that the shape of the resonance peak and the associated oscillations is better described with three filters than with two or more than three. Although the filters can be arranged in any order, I tentatively consider the filters with relatively short time constants, τ1 and τ2, as being involved in the processes of transmitter release (e.g., related to the rate of presynaptic calcium extrusion; Morgans, El Far, Berntson, Wässle, & Taylor, 1998), synaptic diffusion (e.g., related to the rate of glutamate removal; Gaal, Roska, Picaud, Wu, Marc, et al., 1998), or postsynaptic transduction. The longer time constant τh may then be interpreted as the effective time constant of the horizontal cell. The properties of the feedback loop, in particular the total gain, will depend on the spatial properties of the stimulus with respect to the (broad) receptive field of the horizontal cell. Because the spatial extent of all stimuli used in the measurements considered in this article was kept constant (5° diameter), I assume a fixed total gain for each cell. The influence of the spatial layout of the stimulus will be considered in a forth-coming article. 
Figure 5
 
A. The cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit is basically linear (subtractive feedback), where the membrane potential of the horizontal cell, Vh, is subtracted from that of the cone, Vis. The generally small difference is amplified by a gain gs, which forces Vh to follow Vis closely at low frequencies. At intermediate frequencies, the low-pass filters in the loop produce a delay causing oscillations in the response. At high frequencies, the feedback ceases to function, and Vh is then a low-pass filtered version of Vis. B. Nonlinearities added to the loop of A. It(Vs) describes the synaptic activation; aI is a factor depending, through Vis, on the background illuminance.
Figure 5
 
A. The cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit is basically linear (subtractive feedback), where the membrane potential of the horizontal cell, Vh, is subtracted from that of the cone, Vis. The generally small difference is amplified by a gain gs, which forces Vh to follow Vis closely at low frequencies. At intermediate frequencies, the low-pass filters in the loop produce a delay causing oscillations in the response. At high frequencies, the feedback ceases to function, and Vh is then a low-pass filtered version of Vis. B. Nonlinearities added to the loop of A. It(Vs) describes the synaptic activation; aI is a factor depending, through Vis, on the background illuminance.
Nonlinear synaptic gain and illuminance-dependent dynamics
The release of transmitter and its postsynaptic transduction are known to be nonlinear processes. The cone-horizontal cell feedback loop, possibly together with more local ionic feedback circuits, will tend to (apparently) linearize this process (Kraaij, Spekreijse, & Kamermans, 2000). The model as presented up to this point, with a linear gain gs, produces acceptable fits to all horizontal cell measurements considered below. There are two nonlinearities that improved the fits sufficiently to justify their inclusion into the model. For high-contrast steps at high illuminance, there is an indication of a transiently reduced gain gs (see Comparison with measurements, Figure 8). To model this, I included a nonlinearity similar to the one used by Kamermans, Kraaij, and Spekreijse (2001): The effective transmitter release as a function of Vs is, for the values of Vs relevant for the fits, described by a Boltzman function:  
(21)
with gt, Vk, and Vn constants. The more negative Vs becomes, the more the transmitter release will shut down. Because the stimuli considered in this article are spectrally white and have a broad spatial extent (5°), the modulation of Vs is generally small (e.g., see Figure 6B, panel 11). Therefore, the effect of the nonlinearity of Equation 21 is limited. This would be different, however, when the stimulus has a narrow spatial extent, resulting in a larger difference between the response of the excited cones and the horizontal cell, and thus in a larger modulation of Vs. Similarly, a non-white stimulus can increase the modulation of Vs as well. The nonlinearity of Equation 21 is then more important (Kamermans & Spekreijse, 1999). 
Figure 6
 
A. Full model, essentially consisting of a nonlinearity cascaded with two divisive feedback loops and a subtractive feedback loop. B. Response to a 100-ms step in illuminance. The panel numbers correspond to the processing stages marked in A. See the text for details.
Figure 6
 
A. Full model, essentially consisting of a nonlinearity cascaded with two divisive feedback loops and a subtractive feedback loop. B. Response to a 100-ms step in illuminance. The panel numbers correspond to the processing stages marked in A. See the text for details.
Figure 8
 
Effect of using a linear (dashed green line) or nonlinear (continuous red line) synaptic gain for a 100-ms step of contrast 16 at 100 td (i.e., a step from 100 to 1700 td; step starts at t = 0 ms; see black bar at time axis). Parameters for the nonlinear case are identical to those of Figure 7; for the linear case gs = 8.81, with other parameters identical to those of Figure 7.
Figure 8
 
Effect of using a linear (dashed green line) or nonlinear (continuous red line) synaptic gain for a 100-ms step of contrast 16 at 100 td (i.e., a step from 100 to 1700 td; step starts at t = 0 ms; see black bar at time axis). Parameters for the nonlinear case are identical to those of Figure 7; for the linear case gs = 8.81, with other parameters identical to those of Figure 7.
A second nonlinearity becomes clear when inspecting the high-frequency oscillations seen in response to pulses (Figure 7). The frequency of these oscillations decreases with decreasing background intensity. This indicates that the time constants or gain of the feedback loop are intensity dependent. This also influences the high-frequency part of the curves giving the sensitivity as a function of illuminance and frequency (Figure 14). I found that the shift in oscillation frequency could be modeled by assuming that a single factor, aI, decreases the gain gt and increases the time constants τ2 (or, equivalently, τ1) and τh with decreasing intensity. The effective gain and time constants are then gt/aI, aIτ2, and aIτh. Decreasing intensity covaries with increasing Vis; Good fits were obtained by making aI depend on Vis, a low-pass filtered version of Vis, as  
(22)
, with VI and μ constants, and Vis obtained by low-pass filtering Vis with a time constant τa of 250 ms. Similar results were obtained by assuming that aI depends on Vh rather than Vis. The changes in gain and time constants obtained from the fits were modest, typically 5–20% for 10-fold steps of the background intensity. Figure 5B shows the system model associated with both of the above nonlinearities. 
Figure 7
 
Black circles: data from Figures 4 and 5 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). The dashed blue lines show the responses of the model using the generic parameter values (Table 1). The left column shows responses to 10-ms pulses (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes) of varying contrast (0.1, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16) at background illuminances of 100, 10, and 1 td. The right column shows this for 100-ms steps (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes). Traces are offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. Parameters of the fitted curves: τR = 0.49, τE = 16.8, cβ = 2.8·10−3, kβ = 1.63·10−4, τC = 2.89, aC = 9.08·10−2, γ = 0.678, τis = 56.9, ais = 7.09·10−2, gt = 151.1, VI = 19.7, and μ = 0.733; for dimensions of the parameters, see Table 1. Parameters not mentioned are set at the generic values given in Table 1 (as they are for the figures below). Note that the order of the parameter values of τR and τE is arbitrary; the shortest of the values will always be arbitrarily assigned to τR below.
Figure 7
 
Black circles: data from Figures 4 and 5 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). The dashed blue lines show the responses of the model using the generic parameter values (Table 1). The left column shows responses to 10-ms pulses (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes) of varying contrast (0.1, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16) at background illuminances of 100, 10, and 1 td. The right column shows this for 100-ms steps (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes). Traces are offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. Parameters of the fitted curves: τR = 0.49, τE = 16.8, cβ = 2.8·10−3, kβ = 1.63·10−4, τC = 2.89, aC = 9.08·10−2, γ = 0.678, τis = 56.9, ais = 7.09·10−2, gt = 151.1, VI = 19.7, and μ = 0.733; for dimensions of the parameters, see Table 1. Parameters not mentioned are set at the generic values given in Table 1 (as they are for the figures below). Note that the order of the parameter values of τR and τE is arbitrary; the shortest of the values will always be arbitrarily assigned to τR below.
Figure 14
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 2 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The graphs show the amplitude sensitivity in mV/retinal illuminance (top panels) and phase (bottom panels) plotted as a function of frequency for 3 H1 cells (A, B, and C), measured at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.60, τE=8.79, cβ=2.50·10−3, kβ=1.51·10−4, τC=3.24, aC=1.48·10−1, ais=4.08·10−2, gt=126, VI=19.8, and μ=0.57. Fitted parameters of B: τR=6.17, τE=8.33, cβ=1.98·10−3, kβ=1.18·10−4, τC=6.25, aC=1.43·10−1, ais=2.61·10−2, gt=71.2, VI=49.5, and μ=0.24. Fitted parameters of C: τ R=6.50, τE=7.94, cβ=3.32·10−3, kβ=3.15 ·10−4, τC=3.52, aC=1.21·10−1, ais=1.88·10−2, gt=82.5, VI=41.3, and μ=0.17.
Figure 14
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 2 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The graphs show the amplitude sensitivity in mV/retinal illuminance (top panels) and phase (bottom panels) plotted as a function of frequency for 3 H1 cells (A, B, and C), measured at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.60, τE=8.79, cβ=2.50·10−3, kβ=1.51·10−4, τC=3.24, aC=1.48·10−1, ais=4.08·10−2, gt=126, VI=19.8, and μ=0.57. Fitted parameters of B: τR=6.17, τE=8.33, cβ=1.98·10−3, kβ=1.18·10−4, τC=6.25, aC=1.43·10−1, ais=2.61·10−2, gt=71.2, VI=49.5, and μ=0.24. Fitted parameters of C: τ R=6.50, τE=7.94, cβ=3.32·10−3, kβ=3.15 ·10−4, τC=3.52, aC=1.21·10−1, ais=1.88·10−2, gt=82.5, VI=41.3, and μ=0.17.
Example of model performance
The model is shown in its complete form in Figure 6A. Figure 6B shows how a simple stimulus, a 100-ms step of contrast 2 at a background illuminance of 100 td, is trans-formed by the subsequent processing steps in the model, using the parameters fitted to Figure 7 below. The encircled numbers in the panels correspond to the numbers in Figure 6A. The intensity step is first low-pass filtered by τR and τE, yielding a signal proportional to the concentration of activated PDE (panel 2). The rate of cGMP hydrolysis, β, is elevated by cβ relative to the E* curve (Equation 10), hardly visible in the graph at these illuminances (panel 3). The inverse of β (panel 4) is the signal that acts as the input to the calcium feedback loop. It is regulated by 1/α (panel 8), a low-pass filtered version of the loop output. This signal is slightly delayed relative to 1/β because of the low-pass filtering, and therefore boosts high temporal frequencies (panel 5). Furthermore, the feedback effectively reduces the dynamic range needed by the signal (cf. panels 6 and 4). The feedback loop of the inner segment (panel 10) reduces low frequencies (panel 9). In panel 9 the membrane voltage of the cone is shown relative to its membrane potential in the dark (i.e., Vis-Vis,dark). Similarly, panel 14 shows the membrane voltage of the horizontal cell relative to its dark value (i.e., Vh-Vh,dark). It should be noted that the scaling of Vis relative to Vh is not fully determined by the measure-ments on Vh considered here, because it depends on fixing gh = 1 in Equation 20. In the cone-horizontal cell feedback loop (panels 11–14), the membrane potential of the horizontal cell is subtracted from that of the cone. The resulting Vs = Vis-Vh is small, only in the order of a mV. It is transient, because the signal in the horizontal cell is delayed, due to the low-pass filtering, relative to that in the cone. The resulting oscillations (panels 11, 12) arrive, however, strongly attenuated in the horizontal cell (panel 14). Note that the signal presumably going to the bipolar cells (bc in Figure 6A) is considerably more transient (panel 13) than that of the horizontal cell. How much of this shows up in the bipolar cells depends on the amount of low-pass filtering and further processing occurring in the bipolar cells. 
Model implementation
I implemented the model as a series of auto-regressive moving average (ARMA) filters (e.g., Fante, 1988; also see below). A Fortran90 code producing the example of Figure 6B can be obtained by clicking here (Supplementary material. I verified the correctness of the model implementation by comparing its responses to small signals with the transfer function obtained from a small-signal analysis of the model (see Supplementary material, and by comparing its responses to large signals with a fourth-order Runge-Kutta solution of the system of differential equations involved. The implementation using ARMA filters is advantageous because it is both highly accurate and very fast, and therefore allows extensive fitting of the model to the horizontal cell data. The fitting was performed using a simplex algorithm (Press, Teukolsky, Vetterling, & Flannery, 1992) for minimizing the RMS-deviation between model responses and measurements. When responses with very different amplitudes rmax were used for simultaneous fitting, the RMS-deviations were scaled by 1/r0.25max to prevent the largest responses to completely dominate the fit. This was a pragmatic choice that still assigns more weight to large responses because they are less affected by noise and provide more information on the nonlinearities in the model. The measured data from horizontal cells in Figures 714 were obtained from the graphs in the on-line versions of Smith et al. (2001) and Lee et al. (2003), using specialized software (g3data). 
For a first-order low-pass filter with time constant τ, the output y(n) to an input x(n), with a time step Δt, is given by the following ARMA filter (Brown, 2000):  
(23)
with  
(24)
In the calculations I used Δt = 100 μs; with this time step, the model computes approximately 100 times faster than the real cone (3 GHz PC, Intel Fortran compiler on Linux). I verified that results were indistinguishable from those obtained with time steps of 10 μs or 200 μs. For time steps significantly longer than 200 μs, the implicit extra delay of one time step in the high-gain feedback loops of Figure 6A leads to spurious oscillations. 
Comparison with measurements
Below I present fits of the model to a range of measurements on macaque H1 horizontal cells, as published in Lee et al. (2003) and Smith et al. (2001). The behavior of the model is complex, due to the nonlinearities and feedback loops, and the model contains many parameters. There is always the possibility, therefore, that good fits are obtained by overfitting (i.e., with very different, nonphysiological parameter values for different stimuli or different cells). To show that this is not a significant problem here, I determined a set of typical parameter values (listed in Table 1), which together determine a “generic model” producing generic responses. For most measurements presented below (black symbols), I will show two model calculations: the generic response (dashed blue line) and the fitted response (continuous red line). I often fitted with only a subset of parameters left free, fixing the others because which parameters are relevant depends on the type of stimulus. For example, the parameters determining the behavior of the model for large changes of background intensity should not be left free to vary for a medium contrast stimulus at one particular background intensity. Details on the fitted parameters and their values are given in the Figure captions and in Table 1
Table 1
Table 1
Symbol Description Units Generic value Range
τR time constant of R* inactivation ms 3.4 0.5 – 6.5
τE time constant of E* inactivation ms 8.7 3.0 – 16.8
cβ rate constant of cGMP hydrolysis in darkness (ms)−1 2.8·10−3 2.0·10−3 – 4·10−3
kβ rate constant of cGMP hydrolysis (ms)−1 1.6·10−4 4.9·10−5 − 3.9·10−4
β cGMP hydrolysis rate (ms)−1
τX time constant of cGMP turnover ms
X scaled cGMP concentration au
nX apparent Hill coefficient of CNG activation 1 fixed
Ios scaled photocurrent of outer segment au
τC time constant of Ca2+ extrusion ms 3 2 – 6.3
C scaled Ca2+ concentration au
aC scaling constant of GC activation au 9·10−2 3.5·10−2 – 2.1·10−1
nC apparent Hill coefficient of GC activation 4 fixed
α GC activity au
τm capacitive membrane time constant ms 4 fixed
Vis membrane voltage of inner segment mV
γ parameter of membrane nonlinearity 0.7 0.49 – 0.73
αis scaling constant of membrane nonlinearity au 7·10−2 1.9·10−2 – 1.7·10−1
τis time constant of membrane nonlinearity ms 90 23 – 139
Vs effective membrane voltage of cone pedicle after subtractive feedback mV
gt parameter of transmitter activation curve au 125 71 – 185
Vk parameter of transmitter activation curve mV −10 fixed
Vn parameter of transmitter activation curve mV 3 fixed
It transmitter activation au
VI parameter of gain factor aI mV 20 20 – 50
μ parameter of gain factor aI 0.7 0.17 – 0.73
αa time constant for gain factor aI ms 250 fixed
aI gain factor
τ1 time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 4 fixed
τ2 time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 4 2.5 – 4
τh time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 20 20 – 35
Vh membrane voltage of horizontal cell mV
Because the properties of horizontal cells, such as their sensitivity, vary, the generic response will generally deviate from the measurements. But as will become clear below, the generic responses always capture the basic qualifying characteristics of the measured responses. 
Apart from the parameters of the model of Figure 6A, there is one additional parameter that I used as a free parameter in the fits where applicable, namely an overall delay. This accounts for any delay not incorporated into the model (such as transmission and diffusion times and possibly instrumental delay). The delay was forced to be identical for the responses to all stimulus conditions used for a particular cell. It was always small, typically around 3 ms. 
The light intensities in this study are expressed in units of trolands (td). This is a measure of retinal illuminance, defined as the area, in mm2, of the pupil of the eye times the scene luminance, expressed in candela/m2. At a luminance of 100 cd/m2, the pupil has a diameter of approximately 3 mm, thus then 100 cd/m2 corresponds to 640 td. Such a luminance corresponds roughly to the mean luminance outdoors on a dull cloudy day, and is an order of magnitude higher than the typical mean luminance indoors. 
Pulse and step responses
Figure 7 shows responses to 10-ms pulses and 100-ms steps, with various contrasts superimposed on 100-, 10-, and 1-td backgrounds. Note the nonlinearity of response size as a function of contrast and of background intensity. The model fits (continuous red lines) were made to all stimulus conditions simultaneously, and are generally good, considering the wide range of stimulus conditions. The generic model responses (dashed blue lines) capture the qualitative characteristics of the measured responses. 
It is possible to identify the parts of the model responsible for various aspects of the response shapes. The response size as a function of contrast and background intensity is mainly determined by the model components up to the generation of the photocurrent, Ios. The response sagging during the step responses at 100 td, and the response rebounds after pulses and steps, are mainly due to the properties of the model components representing the inner segment. The high-frequency oscillations are due to the cone-horizontal cell feedback loop. 
At 100 td these oscillations are less prominent at high-contrast steps than at low-contrast steps, which points to the existence of a nonlinearity in the cone-horizontal cell feedback loop. This was modeled in Figure 5B. Figure 8 shows the effect of the absence of this nonlinearity (as in the model of Figure 5A). The effect of the nonlinearity can be understood as follows. During a high-contrast step, Vs becomes strongly negative, and thus brings the transmitter release, Equation 21, into a part of the curve with decreased slope. This decreases the small-signal gain, and this decreased gain in the feedback loop leads to a less steep rise of the response and less prominent oscillations. The fact that the oscillations in the model response are not as strongly reduced as in the measured response suggests that there are additional nonlinearities present, possibly due to voltage-sensitive channels in the horizontal cell membrane active at large hyperpolarizations. 
I found that the best fits to the stimuli here and below were obtained with a low value of the time constant of the calcium feedback loop, with τC typically 3 ms. Forcing τC to be much larger worsened the fits considerably, and led to strongly biphasic cone photocurrents and membrane voltages in response to pulses and steps. This biphasic behavior is not consistent with the horizontal cell measurements considered here, which show only a mild rebound (Figure 7), with dynamics differing from those predicted from a slow calcium feedback. In rods, the photocurrent is generally not biphasic, unless the calcium dynamics are manipulated (Torre, Matthews, & Lamb, 1986). Although there are reports in the literature of strongly biphasic photocurrents and membrane voltages in primate cones (Schnapf, Nunn, Meister, & Baylor, 1990; Schneeweis & Schnapf, 1999), these findings may well be a consequence of a disturbed calcium dynamics due to the experimental techniques used. Such a disturbance of the cones is unlikely to have occurred in the horizontal cell measurements considered here, because cones were not directly manipulated, and the preparation left the retina mostly intact (Dacey, 1999; Smith et al., 2001). 
Responses to sinusoids
Figure 9 shows responses to sinusoids of various frequencies at contrasts 0.25, 0.5, and 1, all at a 1000-td background level. In particular at high contrasts there are several distortions visible in the experimental data, which are also produced by the model. The compressive/expansive distortion at 0.61 Hz is mainly produced by the static nonlinearity, 1/β. The low-intensity part of the sinusoidal stimulus produces a small β, which is subsequently blown up by 1/β to the high peak in the response. The peak height is limited by the minimum value of β, cβ in Equation 10. The high-intensity parts of the sinusoid produce a large β, which is subsequently compressed by 1/β, which acts then as a compressive nonlinearity. The detailed shape of the distortion at 0.61 Hz is also determined by the calcium feedback loop, which in effect relinearizes the response to some extent: the high peak (low intensity, large 1/β) produces high levels of calcium, reducing the gain of the forward path (divisive gain control in Figure 3). This brings the response considerably closer to the steady-state level (dashed line). On the other hand, the low response (due to high intensity, small 1/β) produces low levels of calcium, increasing the gain of the forward path, also resulting in a response closer to the steady state than would have resulted without the calcium feedback. Because the former effect is stronger than the latter, the distortion is reduced. 
Figure 9
 
Black circles: data from Figure 6 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Responses are shown to sinusoids of 0.61, 4.88, 9.76, and 30.3 Hz, of contrast 0.25, 0.5, and 1, at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed lines show the steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τR = 3.46, τE = 9.01, cβ = 3.44·10−3,kβ = 6.44·10−5, τC = 2.41, aC = 9.51·10−2, γ = 0.488, τis = 73.2, and ais = 8.68·10−2; fixed parameter: gt = 150.
Figure 9
 
Black circles: data from Figure 6 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Responses are shown to sinusoids of 0.61, 4.88, 9.76, and 30.3 Hz, of contrast 0.25, 0.5, and 1, at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed lines show the steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τR = 3.46, τE = 9.01, cβ = 3.44·10−3,kβ = 6.44·10−5, τC = 2.41, aC = 9.51·10−2, γ = 0.488, τis = 73.2, and ais = 8.68·10−2; fixed parameter: gt = 150.
For higher frequencies, the distortion due to 1/β gradually disappears, because the first two low-pass filters, τR and τE, reduce the depth of modulation of β and thus 1/β. At 4.88 and 9.76 Hz, another distortion becomes clearly visible: The falling flank of the response becomes steeper than the rising flank. This distortion is mainly due to the low-pass filters in the calcium feedback loop: The maximum reduction due to the control signal 1/α (Figure 6A) is only reached right after the peak in 1/β, which results in a steep falling flank right after that peak. 
Finally, a third distortion can be seen at 30.3 Hz, where the rising flank is steeper than the falling flank. This is due to the filtering by the cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit. This circuit has a resonance frequency around 30–40 Hz, with strong phase changes near the resonance peak. This strongly shifts the phase of the distortion products, harmonics at multiples of the fundamental frequency, already produced by the cone. The first harmonic thus gradually shifts phase relative to the fundamental when going through the frequency range surrounding the resonance frequency, which happens to result in a steepening of the rising flank at 30.3 Hz. The distortions described here appear not to be specific to macaque H1 cells, because very similar distortions were measured in cat horizontal cells (Lankheet, van Wezel, & van de Grind, 1991). 
It may be noted that some of the remaining deviations between measurements and fits in Figure 9 are related to small vertical offsets between them. Indeed, the fits improve when allowing for small errors, in the order of a mV or less, in the estimates of the steady-state potentials (horizontal dashed lines). Such errors could arise, for example, by small drifts in the intracellularly recorded membrane potential during the experiment. 
Sinusoid on sinusoid
In Lee et al. (2003) an experiment was performed to test the speed of the sensitivity regulation. It consists of a high-frequency test sinusoid superimposed on a low-frequency vehicle wave of high modulation depth. The lo-cal response to the test modulation then gives a measure of the sensitivity at a particular phase, and therefore illuminance, in the vehicle wave. Figure 10 shows measurements and model responses. As can be seen, the sensitivity regulation is almost instantaneous, with only a small delay mainly due to the low-pass filters in the calcium control loop. The test response can be quantified by extracting the amplitude of the fundamental frequency for each response cycle (Lee et al., 2003). Figure 11 shows for two cells how this response varies as a function of vehicle contrast. Clearly, the responses are well captured by the model. 
Figure 10
 
Black circles: data from Figure 1B of Lee et al. (2003),f itted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A test wave of 19.5 Hz with an amplitude of 127.5 td was superimposed on a vehicle wave of 0.61 Hz and contrast 0.825 at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed line: steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τE=5.10, kβ =1.14·10−4, aC=3.53·10−2, γ =0.729, τis=22.8, andais=6.68·10−2; fixed parameters: τR=1, cβ =3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=100.
Figure 10
 
Black circles: data from Figure 1B of Lee et al. (2003),f itted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A test wave of 19.5 Hz with an amplitude of 127.5 td was superimposed on a vehicle wave of 0.61 Hz and contrast 0.825 at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed line: steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τE=5.10, kβ =1.14·10−4, aC=3.53·10−2, γ =0.729, τis=22.8, andais=6.68·10−2; fixed parameters: τR=1, cβ =3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=100.
Figure 11
 
Black circles: data from Figure 4 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The data points show the amplitude of the first harmonic of the test wave in an experiment similar to the one in Figure 10, for vehicle contrasts of 0.2, 0.34, 0.6, and 0.85. A and B are from two different H1 cells. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.66, τE=9.27, kβ=7.83·10−5, aC=1.09·10−1, and ais=3.33·10−2; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3 τC=4, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.87, τE=2.99, kβ=7.34·10−5, aC=1.54·10−1, and ais=6.09·10−2; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=4, and gt=150.
Figure 11
 
Black circles: data from Figure 4 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The data points show the amplitude of the first harmonic of the test wave in an experiment similar to the one in Figure 10, for vehicle contrasts of 0.2, 0.34, 0.6, and 0.85. A and B are from two different H1 cells. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.66, τE=9.27, kβ=7.83·10−5, aC=1.09·10−1, and ais=3.33·10−2; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3 τC=4, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.87, τE=2.99, kβ=7.34·10−5, aC=1.54·10−1, and ais=6.09·10−2; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=4, and gt=150.
Responsivity and Weber’s law
By dividing the test response amplitude (in mV) by the amplitude of the test stimulus (in troland), a measure is obtained (in mV/td) of the responsivity of the system. Studying the responsivity over a range of background intensities reveals where the system is on the scale from linear to the point where it follows Weber’s law. For a linear system, the responsivity should be independent of background intensity: a stimulus of given strength in td should always produce the same response in mV. For a system following Weber’s law (i.e., a system displaying contrast constancy), the responsiVIty should decline inversely with the back-ground illuminance: The response to 1 td at a background of 100 td (contrast 0.01) should be 10 times smaller than the response to 1 td at a background of 10 td (contrast 0.1). Intermediate stages between linear and Weber are possible as well. 
Figures 12A and B show the results of an experiment designed to test this (Lee et al., 2003). As can be seen, responsivity indeed declines as a function of background intensity, although not as fast as Weber’s law would prescribe. This can be seen better in Figure 12C, where responsivities (at the test frequency of 19.5 Hz) as a function of local il-luminance have been averaged for a series of cells (black symbols, with error bounds shown by the dotted black lines). The continuous red line shows the fitted response; the generic response (dashed blue line) lies within the error bounds of the data. The Figure shows that the system be-haves linearly at low intensities, and only curves toward Weber’s law at higher intensities. On this log-log plot, Weber’s law would correspond to a slope of −1. Both data and model fall in general short of that, only reaching slopes close to −1 at illuminances near 1000 td. At higher retinal illuminances, several thousands of trolands and up, pigment bleaching becomes important and starts contributing to contrast constancy (Hood & Finkelstein, 1986; Pugh & Lamb, 2000). 
Figure 12
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 5A, 5B, and 5C of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Stimuli similar to those in Figure 11 were used, where the response amplitude was now divided by the illuminance amplitude of the test wave to yield a measure of test responsivity (mV/td). The experiment was per-formed at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td for two different H1 cells (panels A and B). C. Similar curves as in A and B were collected for 15 H1 cells, and the momentary test responsivity was plotted as a function of the momentary illuminance of the vehicle wave. The symbols show the average of 15 cells; dotted black lines ±1SD. Fitted parameters of A: τR=2.15, τE=7.59, kβ=2.20·10−4, aC=2.08·10−1, and ais=1.27·10−1; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.08, τE=10.9, kβ=3.86·10−4, aC=1.19·10−1, and ais=1.74·10−1; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of C: kβ=2.01·10−4, aC=1.22·10−1, and ais=8.05·10−2; fixed parameters of C: τR=4, τE=6, cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150.
Figure 12
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 5A, 5B, and 5C of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Stimuli similar to those in Figure 11 were used, where the response amplitude was now divided by the illuminance amplitude of the test wave to yield a measure of test responsivity (mV/td). The experiment was per-formed at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td for two different H1 cells (panels A and B). C. Similar curves as in A and B were collected for 15 H1 cells, and the momentary test responsivity was plotted as a function of the momentary illuminance of the vehicle wave. The symbols show the average of 15 cells; dotted black lines ±1SD. Fitted parameters of A: τR=2.15, τE=7.59, kβ=2.20·10−4, aC=2.08·10−1, and ais=1.27·10−1; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.08, τE=10.9, kβ=3.86·10−4, aC=1.19·10−1, and ais=1.74·10−1; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of C: kβ=2.01·10−4, aC=1.22·10−1, and ais=8.05·10−2; fixed parameters of C: τR=4, τE=6, cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150.
Peak response to increments and decrements
Figures 13A and 13B show responses, at 1000-td back-ground illuminance, to incremental and decremental pulses of several durations and contrasts. Note the asymmetry in shapes of responses to decrements and increments at larger durations, and the changes in shape for different contrasts. Again, these nonlinear aspects are well captured by the model. Keeping the parameter values obtained from the fit to these responses fixed, Figure 13C shows how the model predicts the measured peak responses as a function of pulse duration. Apart from the 6 data points explicitly fitted in Figure 13A and 13B, all other data points are accurately predicted by the model. 
Figure 13
 
Black circles: data from Figures 9 and 10 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A. Responses to 1-ms pulses of contrast −0.9 and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. B. Responses to 64-m spulses of contrast −0.9, −0.5, 0.5, and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. Traces at A and B were offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. C. Peak response for incremental and decremental pulses as a function of contrast (−0.9, −0.5, −0.25, 0.25, 0.5,0.9) and pulse duration. Fitted parameters of A, B, and C: τR=3.64, τE=4.24, kβ=4.92·10−5, aC=6.74·10−2, γ=0.671, τis=139, ais=6.45·10−2, gt=185, and τ2=2.5; fixed parameters of A, B, and C: cβ=4·10−3, τC=2, and τh=35.
Figure 13
 
Black circles: data from Figures 9 and 10 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A. Responses to 1-ms pulses of contrast −0.9 and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. B. Responses to 64-m spulses of contrast −0.9, −0.5, 0.5, and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. Traces at A and B were offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. C. Peak response for incremental and decremental pulses as a function of contrast (−0.9, −0.5, −0.25, 0.25, 0.5,0.9) and pulse duration. Fitted parameters of A, B, and C: τR=3.64, τE=4.24, kβ=4.92·10−5, aC=6.74·10−2, γ=0.671, τis=139, ais=6.45·10−2, gt=185, and τ2=2.5; fixed parameters of A, B, and C: cβ=4·10−3, τC=2, and τh=35.
Sensitivity as a function of frequency
Figure 14 shows for three cells the sensitivity as a function of frequency and background illuminance. The model curves were obtained by simulating the response to low-contrast sinusoids, and subsequently extracting the amplitude and phase of the fundamental frequency in the response. The sensitivity is then defined as the response amplitude per unit illuminance modulation. The main features of the measured curves are well captured by the model: (1) a decrease of responsivity as a function of back-ground level for low frequencies, and no decline at high frequencies; (2) an increase of the cut-off frequency as a function of background level; and (3) a resonance and sharp phase decline at 30–40 Hz, in particular visible at the highest background level. 
The model responses of Figure 14 are obtained using the full nonlinear model, but are in faCt identical to those obtained using a linearized, small-signal version of the model (see Supplementary material. The small-signal analysis also shows that the cGMP hydrolysis by PDE (i.e., 1/β) is the key factor determining the illuminance dependence of responsivity and cut-off frequency. 
Discussion
The model developed here for describing sensitivity regulation in the outer retina performs quite well. It produces the main features of the responses to a wide variety of stimuli, at backgrounds ranging from 1 to 1000 td. When going from low to high illuminances, it produces the measured transition from linear behavior to contrast constancy, as well as a considerable increase in the speed of the response. Moreover, the characteristic distortions of measured responses to sinusoids, indicative of nonlinearities in the processing, are generated in the right way. The model is built from modules that all have a clear physiological interpretation, in particular in terms of the established dynamics of the phototransduction cascade, and the interactions between cones and horizontal cells. 
Notwithstanding the overall success of the model, there are small differences between the modeled and measured responses for most stimuli. This is not too surprising: Several of the components were only modeled in a stylized way. For example, the calcium dynamics may be more complicated due to calcium buffering, and the dynamics of the inner segment membrane over its entire voltage range will not be as simple as modeled. Many of the parameters involved are in fact likely to change somewhat as a function of ion concentrations and membrane potential. For example, the first low-pass filter in the phototransduction cascade, τR, is known or, in cones, at least suspected to depend on the calcium concentration (Pugh & Lamb, 2000). The membrane properties, such as input resistance and time constant, of both cone and horizontal cell are likely to change depending on membrane potential, and thus to change depending on stimulus contrast and background illuminance. For a more precise fitting of the model to the measurements, many of such parameter changes would probably need to be included. However, it appears that such changes can be considered as second-order effects for the present purpose. The model, as it stands, gives an adequate description and explanation of the major response properties, and it is simple and accurate enough to serve as a useful preprocessing stage for models of upstream processing in the retina and beyond. 
The validity of the model is not expected to extend much beyond the illuminance range of the measurements used for validation here (i.e., approximately 1–2000 td). At the high end of this illuminance range, pigment bleaching will become increasingly important, changing the gain of the system and adding a process with particularly slow dynamics. At the low end of the illuminance range, several of the parameters may change: For example, τR may increase, the calcium dynamics may become slower and more complicated, the production rate of cGMP, α as described in Equation 15, may need an extra term αmin (Nikonov et al., 2000) at large calcium concentrations, and the filtering properties of the inner segment may change as well when it is steadily depolarized. Several of these processes could be added to the model under guidance of existing experimental data, but ideally an extended model should be validated to a set of experiments similar to those used here, per-formed at a still larger range of illuminances. 
Much of the model draws on the remarkably detailed picture of the phototransduction cascade in rods that has unfolded in the past decades. Much less is known about the phototransduction cascade in cones, but it is generally assumed that it consists of essentially the same processes as in rods. The model fits obtained here suggest several properties that are perhaps specific to cones or even to cones of primates: The calcium feedback loop is fast (τC ≈3 ms) and powerful (nC ≈ 4), whereas its forward gain is effectively linearized (nX≈ 1), possibly due to the calcium gain control on the CNG channels. For the intensity range considered, the model did not require a calcium-dependent change in the inactivation rate of the cone visual pigment. 
The cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit is similar to circuits proposed and analyzed earlier (for a review, see Kamermans & Spekreijse, 1999). In particular, it is consistent with current ideas that the feedback may be ephaptic (i.e., produced through extracellular currents and voltages in the microenvironment of the cone-horizontal cell-bipolar cell synapse; Kamermans, Fahrenfort et al., 2001). It would be consistent, though, with other mechanisms as well, as long as the feedback is mainly subtractive (i.e., linear). This is an essential requirement to be consistent with the spatially local character of the gain control (Lee, Dacey, Smith, & Pokorny, 1999). The idea is that the major nonlinearities are confined to the cone, and are thus established once the signal has arrived at the intracellular voltage of the cone pedicle. From that point on, the processing is essentially linear (spatiotemporal subtractive feedback), and will thus not influence indicators of local nonlinearities (e.g., spatial distortion products as in MacLeod, Williams, & Makous, 1992) or local gain control (e.g., indicated by the center response amplitude during intensity modulated rings, as in Lee et al., 1999). Note that the nonlinearities in the cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit proposed here (Figure 5B) can be neglected for low-to-medium-contrast stimuli, at least for wide field, spectrally white stimuli, and are anyway small compared with the 1/β nonlinearity of the cone. 
There is a long history of attempts to model sensitiacty regulation at an early processing stage in the vertebrate visual system (e.g., Sperling & Sondhi, 1968; Tranchina, Sneyd, & Cadenas, 1991; Lankheet, van Wezel, Prickaerts, & van de Grind, 1993; He & MacLeod, 1998; Snippe, Poot, & van Hateren, 2000). Usually, these models contain gain control loops and static nonlinearities in various combinations, not unlike the present model. As far as I am aware, the specific form shown in Figure 6A has not been investigated before. The main feature is that it contains a strongly compressive nonlinearity at an early stage, which is subsequently relaxed by an again strongly nonlinear feed-back loop containing an illuminance-dependent time constant. This particular combination of nonlinearities and gain control is well suited for handling the intensities likely to be encountered in the natural environment. The first nonlinearity together with the gain control compresses the large dynamic range of such stimuli into a rather compact range suitable for coding as a membrane potential, and the increased time constant at low intensities averages photon noise at light levels where that is needed. 
The cone-horizontal cell feedback loop produces a highly transient transmitter release at the cone pedicles. This transience is only partly acsible in the responses of the horizontal cell, because this cell low-pass filters its input. For the forward path, via the bipolar cells to the inner retina, this transience is important, and in fact depends on the spatial aspects of the stimulus through the spatiotemporal filtering performed by the horizontal cells. 
The measurements used here for testing the model have been used for developing a quasilinear model by Smith et al. (2001), adding a feedforward gain control loop in Lee et al. (2003). Several of the model components used in the present model have counterparts in their model. The considerable, illuminance-dependent change in the time constant gt1 of their model is reflected in the changing τX in the present model. The model for the inner segment used here behaves, for small signals, approximately as a lead-lag filter (see Supplementary material, similarly to the lead-lag filter used by Smith et al. (2001). The second-order resonator introduced in their model behaves not unlike the implicit resonance of the cone-horizontal cell feedback loop introduced here. Finally, the feedforward gain control of Lee et al. (2003) is to some extent related to the calcium feedback gain control applied here. Nevertheless, the pre-sent model supersedes the earlier attempt, because it en-compasses responses over the entire range of background levels of the measurements at once, including all major nonlinearities, and is physiologically realistic. 
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary File - Small-signal analysis and Weber's law 
Supplementary File - Fortran source code 
Acknowledgments
I wish to thank Herman Snippe for numerous discus-sions, and Herman Snippe and Doekele Stavenga for helpful comments on the manuscript. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: J. H. van Hateren. Email: j.h.van.hateren@rug.nl. 
Address: Dept. of Neurobiophysics, University of Groningen, Nijenborgh 4, NL-9747 AG Groningen, The Netherlands. 
References
Arshavsky, V. Y. Lamb, T. D. Pugh, E. N. (2002). G proteins and phototransduction. Annual Review of Physiology, 64, 153–187. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brown, K. S. (2000). Lead-lag algorithms. http://www .mathpages.com/home/kmath198/kmath198.htm
Burns, M. E. Baylor, D. A. (2001). Activation, deactivation, and adaptation in vertebrate photoreceptor cells. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 779–805. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Burns, M. E. Mendez, A. Chen, J. Baylor, D. A. (2002). Dynamics of cyclic GMP synthesis in retinal rods. Neuron, 36, 81–91. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Calvert, P. D. Govardovskii, V. I. Arshavsky, V. Y. Makino, C. L. (2002). Two temporal phases of light adaptation in retinal rods. Journal of General Physiology, 119, 129–145. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dacey, D. M. (1999). Primate retina: Cell types, circuits and color opponency. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 18, 737–763. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Detwiler, P. B. Hodgkin, A. L. McNaughton, P. A. (1980). Temporal and spatial characteristics of the voltage response of rods in the retina of the snapping turtle. Journal of Physiology, 300, 213–250. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Detwiler, P. B. Ramanathan, S. Sengupta, A. Shrai-man, B. I. (2000). Engineering aspects of enzymatic signal transduction: Photoreceptors in the retina. Biophysical Journal, 79, 2801–2817. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fain, G. L. Matthews, H. R. Cornwall, M. C. Koutalos, Y. (2001). Adaptation in vertebrate photoreceptors. Physiological Reviews, 81, 117–151. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Fante, R. L. (1988) Signal analysis and estimation. New York: Wiley.
Gaal, L. Roska, B. Picaud, S. A. Wu, S. M. Marc, R. Werblin, F. S. (1998). Postsynaptic response kinetics are controlled by a glutamate transporter at cone photoreceptors. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 190–196. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Hamer, R. D. (2000). Computational analysis of vertebrate phototransduction: Combined quantitative and qualitative modeling of dark- and light-adapted responses in amphibian rods. Visual Neuroscience, 17, 679–699. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
He, S. MacLeod, D. I. A. (1998). Contrast-modulation flicker: Dynamics and spatial resolution of the light adaptation process. Vision Research, 38, 985–1000. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hood, D. C. Finkelstein, M. A. Boff, K. R. Kaufman, L. Thomas, J. P. (1986). Sensitivity to light Handbook of perception and human performance. New York: John Wiley.
Hsu, A. Tsukamoto, Y. Smith, R. G. Sterling, P. (1998). Functional architecture of primate cone and rod axons. Vision Research, 38, 2539–2549. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kamermans, M. Fahrenfort, I. Schultz, K. Janssen-Bienhold, U. Sjoerdsma, T. Weiler, R. (2001). Hemichannel-mediated inhibition in the outer retina. Science, 292, 1178–1180. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kamermans, M. Kraaij, D. Spekreijse, H. (2001). The dynamic characteristics of the feedback signal from horizontal cells to cones in the goldfish retina. Journal of Physiology, 534, 489–500. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kamermans, M. Spekreijse, H. (1999). The feedback pathway from horizontal cells to cones: A mini review with a look ahead. Vision Research, 39, 2449–2468. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Koutalos, Y. Yau, K. W. (1996). Regulation of sensitivity in vertebrate rod photoreceptors by calcium. Trends in Neurosciences, 19, 73–81. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kraaij, D. A. Spekreijse, H. Kamermans, M. (2000). The open- and closed-loop gain-characteristics of the cone/horizontal cell synapse in goldfish retina. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 1256–1265. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Lamb, T. D. Pugh, E. N.Jr. (1992). A quantitative Account of the Activation steps involved in phototransduction in amphibian photoreceptors. Journal of Physiology, 449, 719–758. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lankheet, M. J. M. van Wezel, R. J. A. van de Grind, W. A. (1991). Light adaptation and frequency transfer properties of cat horizontal cells. Vision Research, 31, 1129–1142. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lankheet, M. J. M. van Wezel, R. J. A. Prickaerts, J. H. H. J. van de Grind, W. A. (1993). The dynamics of light adaptation in cat horizontal cell responses. Vision Research, 33, 1153–1171. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lee, B. B. Dacey, D. M. Smith, V. C. Pokorny, J. (1999). Horizontal cells reveal cone type-specific adaptation in primate retina. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science U.S.A., 96, 14611–14616. [PubMed][Article] [CrossRef]
Lee, B. B. Dacey, D. M. Smith, V. C. Pokorny, J. (2003). Dynamics of sensitivity regulation in primate outer retina: The horizontal cell network. Journal of Vision, 3(7), 513–526, http://journalofvision.org/3/7/5/, doi:10.1167/3.7.5. [PubMed][Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MacLeod, D. I. A. Williams, D. R. Makous, W. (1992). A visual nonlinearity fed by single cones. Vision Research, 32, 347–363. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Morgans, C. W. El Far, O. Berntson, A. Wässle, H. Taylor, W. R. (1998). Calcium extrusion from mammalian photoreceptor terminals. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2467–2474. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Nikonov, S. Engheta, H. Pugh, E. N.Jr. (1998). Kinetics of recovery of the dark-adapted salamander rod photoresponse. Journal of General Physiology, 111, 7–37. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nikonov, S. Lamb, T. D. Pugh, E. N.Jr. (2000). The role of steady phosphodiesterase Activity in the kinetics and sensitivity of the light-adapted salamander rod photoresponse. Journal of General Physiology, 116, 795–824. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Press, W. H. Teukolsky, S. A. Vetterling, W. T. Flannery, B. P. (1992). Numerical recipes in Fortran. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Pugh, E. N.Jr. Lamb, T. D. Stavenga, D. G. de Grip, W. J. Pugh, E. N.Jr. (2000). Phototransduction in vertebrate rods and cones: Molecular mechanisms of amplification, recovery and light adaptation Handbook of Biological Physics, Vol. 3 (pp. 183–254). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Rebrik, T. I. Korenbrot, J. I. (2004). In intact mammalian photoreceptors, Ca2+-dependent modulation of cGMP-gated ion channels is detectable in cones but not in rods, Journal of General Physiology, 123, 63–75. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schnapf, J. L. Nunn, B. J. Meister, M. Baylor, D. A. (1990). Visual transduction in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Journal of Physiology, 427, 681–713. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schneeweis, D. M. Schnapf, J. L. (1999). The photo-voltage of macaque cone photoreceptors: Adaptation, noise, and kinetics. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 1203–1216. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Smith, V. C. Pokorny, J. Lee, B. B. Dacey, D. M. (2001). Primate horizontal cell dynamics: An analysis of sensitivity regulation in the outer retina. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85, 545–558. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Snippe, H. P. Poot, L. van Hateren, J. H. (2000). A temporal model for early vision that explains detection thresholds for light pulses on flickering backgrounds. Visual Neuroscience, 17, 449–462. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sperling, G. Sondhi, M. M. (1968). Model for visual luminance discrimination and flicker detection. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 58, 1133–1145. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Torre, V. Matthews, H. R. Lamb, T. D. (1986). Role of calcium in regulating the cyclicgmp cascade of photo-transduction in retinal rods. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 83, 7109–7113. [PubMed][[Article] [CrossRef]
Tranchina, D. Sneyd, J. Cadenas, I. D. (1991). Light adaptation in turtle cones: Testing and analysis of a model for phototransduction. Biophysical Journal, 60, 217–237. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yagi, T. MacLeish, P. R. (1994). Ionic conductances of monkey solitary cone inner segments. Journal of Neurophysiology, 71, 656–665. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Figure 1
 
A. Representation of a first-order low-pass filter transforming x(t) into y(t), with time constant τy and DC-gain 1. B. Astatic nonlinear function f(x) transforms x into y for each t.
Figure 1
 
A. Representation of a first-order low-pass filter transforming x(t) into y(t), with time constant τy and DC-gain 1. B. Astatic nonlinear function f(x) transforms x into y for each t.
Figure 2
 
A. Light I produces excited visual pigment R*, which produces excited G-protein G*, activating phosphodiesterase (PDE). The system diagram to the right describes only the associated low-pass filtering, whereas the gain of the reactions is incorporated into the constant kβ shown in B. B. Production of X (cGMP) under conditions of clamped calcium (i.e., fixed α). The differential equation describing production and removal of cGMPcan be interpreted as shown in the system diagram on the right: a static nonlinearity 1/β followed by low-pass filtering with a time constant τX = 1/β.
Figure 2
 
A. Light I produces excited visual pigment R*, which produces excited G-protein G*, activating phosphodiesterase (PDE). The system diagram to the right describes only the associated low-pass filtering, whereas the gain of the reactions is incorporated into the constant kβ shown in B. B. Production of X (cGMP) under conditions of clamped calcium (i.e., fixed α). The differential equation describing production and removal of cGMPcan be interpreted as shown in the system diagram on the right: a static nonlinearity 1/β followed by low-pass filtering with a time constant τX = 1/β.
Figure 3
 
In the calcium control loop, an increase in cGMP (X) opens more membrane channels, increasing the photocurrent Ios and the calcium influx. The increase in calcium reduces, via guanylate cyclase (GC), the production of cGMP, counteracting the initial rise. In a secondary negative feedback loop, calcium reduces the effectiveness of the membrane channels (dashed lines).
Figure 3
 
In the calcium control loop, an increase in cGMP (X) opens more membrane channels, increasing the photocurrent Ios and the calcium influx. The increase in calcium reduces, via guanylate cyclase (GC), the production of cGMP, counteracting the initial rise. In a secondary negative feedback loop, calcium reduces the effectiveness of the membrane channels (dashed lines).
Figure 4
 
Model of the filtering properties of the inner segment.
Figure 4
 
Model of the filtering properties of the inner segment.
Figure 5
 
A. The cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit is basically linear (subtractive feedback), where the membrane potential of the horizontal cell, Vh, is subtracted from that of the cone, Vis. The generally small difference is amplified by a gain gs, which forces Vh to follow Vis closely at low frequencies. At intermediate frequencies, the low-pass filters in the loop produce a delay causing oscillations in the response. At high frequencies, the feedback ceases to function, and Vh is then a low-pass filtered version of Vis. B. Nonlinearities added to the loop of A. It(Vs) describes the synaptic activation; aI is a factor depending, through Vis, on the background illuminance.
Figure 5
 
A. The cone-horizontal cell feedback circuit is basically linear (subtractive feedback), where the membrane potential of the horizontal cell, Vh, is subtracted from that of the cone, Vis. The generally small difference is amplified by a gain gs, which forces Vh to follow Vis closely at low frequencies. At intermediate frequencies, the low-pass filters in the loop produce a delay causing oscillations in the response. At high frequencies, the feedback ceases to function, and Vh is then a low-pass filtered version of Vis. B. Nonlinearities added to the loop of A. It(Vs) describes the synaptic activation; aI is a factor depending, through Vis, on the background illuminance.
Figure 6
 
A. Full model, essentially consisting of a nonlinearity cascaded with two divisive feedback loops and a subtractive feedback loop. B. Response to a 100-ms step in illuminance. The panel numbers correspond to the processing stages marked in A. See the text for details.
Figure 6
 
A. Full model, essentially consisting of a nonlinearity cascaded with two divisive feedback loops and a subtractive feedback loop. B. Response to a 100-ms step in illuminance. The panel numbers correspond to the processing stages marked in A. See the text for details.
Figure 8
 
Effect of using a linear (dashed green line) or nonlinear (continuous red line) synaptic gain for a 100-ms step of contrast 16 at 100 td (i.e., a step from 100 to 1700 td; step starts at t = 0 ms; see black bar at time axis). Parameters for the nonlinear case are identical to those of Figure 7; for the linear case gs = 8.81, with other parameters identical to those of Figure 7.
Figure 8
 
Effect of using a linear (dashed green line) or nonlinear (continuous red line) synaptic gain for a 100-ms step of contrast 16 at 100 td (i.e., a step from 100 to 1700 td; step starts at t = 0 ms; see black bar at time axis). Parameters for the nonlinear case are identical to those of Figure 7; for the linear case gs = 8.81, with other parameters identical to those of Figure 7.
Figure 7
 
Black circles: data from Figures 4 and 5 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). The dashed blue lines show the responses of the model using the generic parameter values (Table 1). The left column shows responses to 10-ms pulses (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes) of varying contrast (0.1, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16) at background illuminances of 100, 10, and 1 td. The right column shows this for 100-ms steps (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes). Traces are offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. Parameters of the fitted curves: τR = 0.49, τE = 16.8, cβ = 2.8·10−3, kβ = 1.63·10−4, τC = 2.89, aC = 9.08·10−2, γ = 0.678, τis = 56.9, ais = 7.09·10−2, gt = 151.1, VI = 19.7, and μ = 0.733; for dimensions of the parameters, see Table 1. Parameters not mentioned are set at the generic values given in Table 1 (as they are for the figures below). Note that the order of the parameter values of τR and τE is arbitrary; the shortest of the values will always be arbitrarily assigned to τR below.
Figure 7
 
Black circles: data from Figures 4 and 5 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). The dashed blue lines show the responses of the model using the generic parameter values (Table 1). The left column shows responses to 10-ms pulses (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes) of varying contrast (0.1, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16) at background illuminances of 100, 10, and 1 td. The right column shows this for 100-ms steps (starting at t=0 ms; see black bars at time axes). Traces are offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. Parameters of the fitted curves: τR = 0.49, τE = 16.8, cβ = 2.8·10−3, kβ = 1.63·10−4, τC = 2.89, aC = 9.08·10−2, γ = 0.678, τis = 56.9, ais = 7.09·10−2, gt = 151.1, VI = 19.7, and μ = 0.733; for dimensions of the parameters, see Table 1. Parameters not mentioned are set at the generic values given in Table 1 (as they are for the figures below). Note that the order of the parameter values of τR and τE is arbitrary; the shortest of the values will always be arbitrarily assigned to τR below.
Figure 14
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 2 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The graphs show the amplitude sensitivity in mV/retinal illuminance (top panels) and phase (bottom panels) plotted as a function of frequency for 3 H1 cells (A, B, and C), measured at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.60, τE=8.79, cβ=2.50·10−3, kβ=1.51·10−4, τC=3.24, aC=1.48·10−1, ais=4.08·10−2, gt=126, VI=19.8, and μ=0.57. Fitted parameters of B: τR=6.17, τE=8.33, cβ=1.98·10−3, kβ=1.18·10−4, τC=6.25, aC=1.43·10−1, ais=2.61·10−2, gt=71.2, VI=49.5, and μ=0.24. Fitted parameters of C: τ R=6.50, τE=7.94, cβ=3.32·10−3, kβ=3.15 ·10−4, τC=3.52, aC=1.21·10−1, ais=1.88·10−2, gt=82.5, VI=41.3, and μ=0.17.
Figure 14
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 2 of Smith et al. (2001), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The graphs show the amplitude sensitivity in mV/retinal illuminance (top panels) and phase (bottom panels) plotted as a function of frequency for 3 H1 cells (A, B, and C), measured at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.60, τE=8.79, cβ=2.50·10−3, kβ=1.51·10−4, τC=3.24, aC=1.48·10−1, ais=4.08·10−2, gt=126, VI=19.8, and μ=0.57. Fitted parameters of B: τR=6.17, τE=8.33, cβ=1.98·10−3, kβ=1.18·10−4, τC=6.25, aC=1.43·10−1, ais=2.61·10−2, gt=71.2, VI=49.5, and μ=0.24. Fitted parameters of C: τ R=6.50, τE=7.94, cβ=3.32·10−3, kβ=3.15 ·10−4, τC=3.52, aC=1.21·10−1, ais=1.88·10−2, gt=82.5, VI=41.3, and μ=0.17.
Figure 9
 
Black circles: data from Figure 6 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Responses are shown to sinusoids of 0.61, 4.88, 9.76, and 30.3 Hz, of contrast 0.25, 0.5, and 1, at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed lines show the steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τR = 3.46, τE = 9.01, cβ = 3.44·10−3,kβ = 6.44·10−5, τC = 2.41, aC = 9.51·10−2, γ = 0.488, τis = 73.2, and ais = 8.68·10−2; fixed parameter: gt = 150.
Figure 9
 
Black circles: data from Figure 6 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Responses are shown to sinusoids of 0.61, 4.88, 9.76, and 30.3 Hz, of contrast 0.25, 0.5, and 1, at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed lines show the steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τR = 3.46, τE = 9.01, cβ = 3.44·10−3,kβ = 6.44·10−5, τC = 2.41, aC = 9.51·10−2, γ = 0.488, τis = 73.2, and ais = 8.68·10−2; fixed parameter: gt = 150.
Figure 10
 
Black circles: data from Figure 1B of Lee et al. (2003),f itted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A test wave of 19.5 Hz with an amplitude of 127.5 td was superimposed on a vehicle wave of 0.61 Hz and contrast 0.825 at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed line: steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τE=5.10, kβ =1.14·10−4, aC=3.53·10−2, γ =0.729, τis=22.8, andais=6.68·10−2; fixed parameters: τR=1, cβ =3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=100.
Figure 10
 
Black circles: data from Figure 1B of Lee et al. (2003),f itted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A test wave of 19.5 Hz with an amplitude of 127.5 td was superimposed on a vehicle wave of 0.61 Hz and contrast 0.825 at a mean illuminance of 1000 td. Horizontal dashed line: steady-state membrane potential. Fitted parameters: τE=5.10, kβ =1.14·10−4, aC=3.53·10−2, γ =0.729, τis=22.8, andais=6.68·10−2; fixed parameters: τR=1, cβ =3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=100.
Figure 11
 
Black circles: data from Figure 4 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The data points show the amplitude of the first harmonic of the test wave in an experiment similar to the one in Figure 10, for vehicle contrasts of 0.2, 0.34, 0.6, and 0.85. A and B are from two different H1 cells. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.66, τE=9.27, kβ=7.83·10−5, aC=1.09·10−1, and ais=3.33·10−2; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3 τC=4, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.87, τE=2.99, kβ=7.34·10−5, aC=1.54·10−1, and ais=6.09·10−2; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=4, and gt=150.
Figure 11
 
Black circles: data from Figure 4 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. The data points show the amplitude of the first harmonic of the test wave in an experiment similar to the one in Figure 10, for vehicle contrasts of 0.2, 0.34, 0.6, and 0.85. A and B are from two different H1 cells. Fitted parameters of A: τR=4.66, τE=9.27, kβ=7.83·10−5, aC=1.09·10−1, and ais=3.33·10−2; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3 τC=4, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.87, τE=2.99, kβ=7.34·10−5, aC=1.54·10−1, and ais=6.09·10−2; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=4, and gt=150.
Figure 12
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 5A, 5B, and 5C of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Stimuli similar to those in Figure 11 were used, where the response amplitude was now divided by the illuminance amplitude of the test wave to yield a measure of test responsivity (mV/td). The experiment was per-formed at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td for two different H1 cells (panels A and B). C. Similar curves as in A and B were collected for 15 H1 cells, and the momentary test responsivity was plotted as a function of the momentary illuminance of the vehicle wave. The symbols show the average of 15 cells; dotted black lines ±1SD. Fitted parameters of A: τR=2.15, τE=7.59, kβ=2.20·10−4, aC=2.08·10−1, and ais=1.27·10−1; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.08, τE=10.9, kβ=3.86·10−4, aC=1.19·10−1, and ais=1.74·10−1; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of C: kβ=2.01·10−4, aC=1.22·10−1, and ais=8.05·10−2; fixed parameters of C: τR=4, τE=6, cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150.
Figure 12
 
Black symbols: data from Figure 5A, 5B, and 5C of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. Stimuli similar to those in Figure 11 were used, where the response amplitude was now divided by the illuminance amplitude of the test wave to yield a measure of test responsivity (mV/td). The experiment was per-formed at background illuminances of 10, 100, and 1000 td for two different H1 cells (panels A and B). C. Similar curves as in A and B were collected for 15 H1 cells, and the momentary test responsivity was plotted as a function of the momentary illuminance of the vehicle wave. The symbols show the average of 15 cells; dotted black lines ±1SD. Fitted parameters of A: τR=2.15, τE=7.59, kβ=2.20·10−4, aC=2.08·10−1, and ais=1.27·10−1; fixed parameters of A: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of B: τR=2.08, τE=10.9, kβ=3.86·10−4, aC=1.19·10−1, and ais=1.74·10−1; fixed parameters of B: cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150. Fitted parameters of C: kβ=2.01·10−4, aC=1.22·10−1, and ais=8.05·10−2; fixed parameters of C: τR=4, τE=6, cβ=3·10−3, τC=2, and gt=150.
Figure 13
 
Black circles: data from Figures 9 and 10 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A. Responses to 1-ms pulses of contrast −0.9 and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. B. Responses to 64-m spulses of contrast −0.9, −0.5, 0.5, and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. Traces at A and B were offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. C. Peak response for incremental and decremental pulses as a function of contrast (−0.9, −0.5, −0.25, 0.25, 0.5,0.9) and pulse duration. Fitted parameters of A, B, and C: τR=3.64, τE=4.24, kβ=4.92·10−5, aC=6.74·10−2, γ=0.671, τis=139, ais=6.45·10−2, gt=185, and τ2=2.5; fixed parameters of A, B, and C: cβ=4·10−3, τC=2, and τh=35.
Figure 13
 
Black circles: data from Figures 9 and 10 of Lee et al. (2003), fitted by the model (continuous red lines). Dashed blue lines: generic model. A. Responses to 1-ms pulses of contrast −0.9 and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. B. Responses to 64-m spulses of contrast −0.9, −0.5, 0.5, and 0.9 at a background of 1000 td. Traces at A and B were offset from 0 mV for the sake of clarity. C. Peak response for incremental and decremental pulses as a function of contrast (−0.9, −0.5, −0.25, 0.25, 0.5,0.9) and pulse duration. Fitted parameters of A, B, and C: τR=3.64, τE=4.24, kβ=4.92·10−5, aC=6.74·10−2, γ=0.671, τis=139, ais=6.45·10−2, gt=185, and τ2=2.5; fixed parameters of A, B, and C: cβ=4·10−3, τC=2, and τh=35.
Table 1
Table 1
Symbol Description Units Generic value Range
τR time constant of R* inactivation ms 3.4 0.5 – 6.5
τE time constant of E* inactivation ms 8.7 3.0 – 16.8
cβ rate constant of cGMP hydrolysis in darkness (ms)−1 2.8·10−3 2.0·10−3 – 4·10−3
kβ rate constant of cGMP hydrolysis (ms)−1 1.6·10−4 4.9·10−5 − 3.9·10−4
β cGMP hydrolysis rate (ms)−1
τX time constant of cGMP turnover ms
X scaled cGMP concentration au
nX apparent Hill coefficient of CNG activation 1 fixed
Ios scaled photocurrent of outer segment au
τC time constant of Ca2+ extrusion ms 3 2 – 6.3
C scaled Ca2+ concentration au
aC scaling constant of GC activation au 9·10−2 3.5·10−2 – 2.1·10−1
nC apparent Hill coefficient of GC activation 4 fixed
α GC activity au
τm capacitive membrane time constant ms 4 fixed
Vis membrane voltage of inner segment mV
γ parameter of membrane nonlinearity 0.7 0.49 – 0.73
αis scaling constant of membrane nonlinearity au 7·10−2 1.9·10−2 – 1.7·10−1
τis time constant of membrane nonlinearity ms 90 23 – 139
Vs effective membrane voltage of cone pedicle after subtractive feedback mV
gt parameter of transmitter activation curve au 125 71 – 185
Vk parameter of transmitter activation curve mV −10 fixed
Vn parameter of transmitter activation curve mV 3 fixed
It transmitter activation au
VI parameter of gain factor aI mV 20 20 – 50
μ parameter of gain factor aI 0.7 0.17 – 0.73
αa time constant for gain factor aI ms 250 fixed
aI gain factor
τ1 time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 4 fixed
τ2 time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 4 2.5 – 4
τh time constant of cone — horizontal cell loop ms 20 20 – 35
Vh membrane voltage of horizontal cell mV
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×