More than 40 years ago, Barlow (
1961) introduced the hypothesis that visual cells are optimized to process natural stimuli. Since then, several investigators have provided support to this natural-adaptation hypothesis (Dan, Attick, & Reid,
1996; Simoncelli & Olshausen,
2001; Srinivasan, Laughlin, & Dubs,
1982) and have shown that natural images emphasize features of responses not prominent when using synthetic stimuli (David, Vinje, & Gallant,
2004). However, only recently, a number of studies have begun to use nonlinear models and natural stimuli to characterize the response of visual cells. These studies obtained interesting findings, but the methods had limitations for investigators interested in general receptive fields. Some groups did not attempt to map receptive fields (Aggelopoulos, Franco, & Rolls,
2005; Dan et al.,
1996; Guo, Robertson, Mahmoodi, & Young,
2005; Vinje & Gallant,
2000,
2002; Weliky, Fiser, Hunt, & Wagner,
2003), whereas others limited their studies to linear (Ringach, Hawken, & Shapley,
2002; Smyth, Willmore, Baker, Thompson, & Tolhurst,
2003; Theunissen et al.,
2001; Willmore & Smyth,
2003) or second-order, nonlinear (Felsen, Touryan, Han, & Dan,
2005; Touryan, Felsen, & Dan,
2005) receptive fields. More general nonlinearities were first studied by modeling cell responses as a linear filter followed by a point nonlinearity (Chichilnisky,
2001; Nykamp & Ringach,
2002) or by fitting a priori models (David et al.,
2004). The former models cannot capture nonlinear interactions between subregions of the receptive field and, thus, might not be sufficiently general to characterize responses of large classes of cells. Later, two methods that make no assumptions on the response-generation mechanism were introduced (Prenger, Wu, David, & Gallant,
2004; Sharpee, Rust, & Bialek,
2004; Sharpee et al.,
2006). These methods are powerful but require delicate nonlinear optimizations that could be overwhelming to many investigators.