Disparity thresholds for simple gratings rise rapidly when a large pedestal is increased further, above phase disparities of about 60°. For compound gratings, large-pedestal thresholds can exceed the highest of the components thresholds. Such interference has been seen previously in the two-frequency increment thresholds of Smallman and MacLeod (
1997), and may have contributed to the steep threshold increases typically seen at large pedestals in previous studies of disparity increments. The scale or spacing of spectral components may be important for this interference effect, because it is not evident in the data for 2+4-c/d gratings.
It might be supposed that interference occurred when compound gratings weren’t fused. At large disparities, squarewave gratings appear as diplopic, whereas sinewave gratings of the same fundamental frequency are seen as single (Kulikowski,
1978). So, the high frequencies in our compound gratings might have narrowed the fusional range, beyond which diplopia might have elevated thresholds. However, Rohaly and Wilson (
1993) found that diplopia thresholds for superimposed D6 patterns, one of high frequency and one of low, were generally as high as the threshold for the low-frequency D6 alone (and in one case actually higher), provided that both patterns had the same disparity. This provision held in our study — all components had the same spatial disparity — and neither diplopia nor depth reversals were noted by our observers. Moreover, our RDSs were outside Panum’s area at the largest pedestals, yet thresholds remained modest.
RDSs may escape interference because of their low-frequency components, below the 0.5 c/d of the compound gratings, because of their aperiodic spatial structure, or because of the continuity of their spatial-frequency spectra. It is known that the presence of low frequencies, as well as high, contributes to fine stereoacuity for broadband patterns (Westheimer & McKee,
1980). Low-frequency contrast-envelope disparities can extend the disparity range of higher-frequency carrier components (McKee, Verghese, & Farell,
2004; Stelmach & Buckthought,
2003). However, in our study, the low frequencies would be those of the RDS pattern; the envelope’s disparity was fixed at zero. Still, the envelope is a possible source of interference. Specifically, the zero-disparity signal of the gratings’ Gaussian envelope, possibly transduced by a second-order pathway (McKee et al.,
2004), could have interacted with the nonzero disparity signal of the low-frequency (0.5 c/d) component of the compound grating. The 2+4-c/d grating could have escaped interactions with the envelope because of its higher-frequency range. However, this leaves unanswered the question of why the large-pedestal thresholds for the 0.5-c/d grating are any less than those for the 0.5+2-, 0.5+4-, and 0.5+2+4-c/d gratings. Whatever the source of this interference effect, thresholds for RDSs and 2+4-c/d gratings appear not to be influenced by it. Therefore, the interference seen in the data for most of the compound gratings is not a general property of disparity discriminations of multi-scale stimuli.