This asymmetry may reflect real-world constraints. There is a fixed lower limit to how much sooner a sound can arrive than vision, as vision involves a relatively slow transduction process compared with audition. Acoustic transduction between the outer and inner ears is a direct and fast mechanical process, taking 1 ms or less (Corey & Hudspeth,
1979; King & Palmer,
1985). Retinal phototransduction is a relatively slow photochemical process followed by several cascading neurochemical stages, lasting around 50 ms (Bolz, Rosner, & Wassle,
1982; Lamb & Pugh,
1992; Lennie,
1981; Rodieck,
1998; Schnapf, Kraft, & Baylor,
1987). Studies of audiovisual temporal alignment have generally found that an auditory stimulus needs to be delayed to be perceptually aligned with a visual stimulus (Bald, Berrien, Price, & Sprague,
1942; Bushara, Grafman, & Hallett,
2001; Hamlin,
1895; Hirsh & Sherrick,
1961; Lewkowicz,
1996; Rutschmann & Link,
1964). Thus, for near-field presentations, where auditory travel time is negligible, sounds will become perceptually available before visual stimuli by several tens of milliseconds. However, as sound source distance increases, this asynchrony decreases, and for distances of 10–15 m, acoustic and visual signals probably become perceptually available at approximately the same time. However, for distances beyond this point, sounds will always become available perceptually later than vision, and there is no upper limit to this. Thus, the broad positive extent of the delay range could reflect this real-world constraint.