In this study, we assessed the temporal dynamics of selective attention with classification images (Ahumada & Lovell,
1971). The classification image technique (Ahumada & Lovell,
1971, see also Eckstein & Ahumada,
2002; Gold, Shiffrin, & Elder,
2006) estimates the spatial information used in a task by assessing how response outcomes depend upon the external or image noise added to the stimulus. For example, a typical classification image analysis of a yes/no detection task assumes that the false alarms (erroneous “yes” responses on no signal trials) are driven at least partly by the external noise. The “false alarm” classification image is the average of the noise fields leading to false alarms and represents the overall information profile the observer used to judge signal presence. It is the behavioral equivalent of the reverse correlation technique that has been used to assess the response characteristics of single neurons (DeAngelis, Ohzawa, & Freeman,
1993a,
1993b; Marmarelis & Marmarelis,
1978; Nykamp & Ringach,
2002; Reid, Victor, & Shapley,
1997; Ringach, Sapiro, & Shapley,
1997). Under linear model assumptions, the classification image is a direct estimate of the template (or filter) an observer uses in a particular visual task (Ahumada,
2002; Murray, Bennett, & Sekuler,
2002). However, an assumption of linearity may not be appropriate in all cases (for discussions of nonlinearity, see Abbey & Eckstein,
2002,
2006; Neri,
2004; Tjan & Nandy,
2006; Victor,
2005). Recently, this technique has been employed to assess a variety of visual phenomena, including Vernier acuity (Ahumada,
1996; Beard & Ahumada,
1998) and other position discrimination judgments (Levi & Klein,
2002), Kaniza squares and other illusory contours (Gold, Murray, Bennett, & Sekuler,
2000), contrast effects (Dakin & Bex,
2003; Shimozaki, Eckstein, & Abbey,
2005), saccadic decisions (Rajashekar, Bovik, & Cormack,
2006), and visual attention (Eckstein, Pham, & Shimozaki,
2004; Eckstein, Shimozaki, & Abbey,
2002).