The goal of human visual processing is to generate functional information from the natural environment based on retinal images that have characteristic and broad distributions of spatial frequency (SF; Bex & Makous,
2002; Bex, Dakin, & Mareschal,
2005; Billock,
1996; Burton & Moorhead,
1987; Field,
1987; Hancock, Baddeley, & Smith,
1992; Tolhurst, Tadmor, & Chao,
1992; Ruderman,
1994; van der Schaaf & van Hateren,
1996; van Hateren & van der Schaaf,
1998) and orientation (Betsch, Einhäuser, Körding, & König,
2004; Coppola, Purves, McCoy, & Purves
1998; Hancock et al.,
1992; Hansen, Essock, Zheng, & Deford,
2003; Keil & Cristóbal,
2000; Switkes, Mayer, & Sloan,
1978; van der Schaaf & van Hateren,
1996) are dynamic (Bex et al.,
2005; Billock, de Guzman, & Kelso,
2001; Dong & Atick,
1995; van Hateren,
1997) and contain variable luminances and contrasts (Balboa & Grzywacz,
2000,
2003; Frazor & Geisler,
2006; Mante, Frazor, Bonin, Geisler, & Carandini,
2005; Ruderman & Bialek,
1994). Much of our understanding of visual processing is based on the results of experiments employing sine wave grating stimuli that are narrow in SF and orientation content and are presented at uniform, barely visible contrasts (Campbell & Green,
1965; Campbell & Robson,
1968). These data have been used to develop widely accepted models of early vision in which the sensitivity of a set of log-scaled SF-selective neurons (Field & Tolhurst,
1986; Lennie & Movshon,
2005; Ringach, Hawken, & Shapley,
2002) or channels (Blakemore & Campbell,
1969; Campbell & Robson,
1968; Graham & Nachmias,
1971) follows the inverted-U shape of the contrast sensitivity function (CSF). At suprathreshold contrasts, inhibitory connections attenuate neural responses through
contrast gain control (Bonds,
1989; Carandini, Heeger, & Movshon,
1997; Geisler & Albrecht,
1992; Heeger,
1992; Morrone, Burr, & Maffei,
1982).