The experiment used four tasks, referred to as the vision-only, audition-only, and vision–audition training tasks, and the vision–audition test task. The vision-only and audition-only tasks allowed us to characterize each subject's performances on visual and auditory discrimination tasks, respectively. The goal of the vision–audition training task was to expose subjects to an environment in which an auditory signal is correlated with a visual cue to motion direction. The goal of the vision–audition test task was to evaluate whether subjects learned that the auditory signal is also a cue to motion direction.
In each trial of the vision-only training task, four visual displays were presented: A fixation square was presented for 500 ms, followed by the first RDK for 1,000 ms, followed by a second fixation square for 400 ms, followed by the second RDK for 1,000 ms. The stimulus direction of the first RDK, referred to as the “standard” stimulus, was always 0° (vertical). The second RDK, referred to as the “comparison” stimulus, had a stimulus direction different from the standard. Subjects judged whether the dots in the comparison stimulus moved to the left (anticlockwise) or to the right (clockwise) of those in the standard (vertical) stimulus. They responded by pressing the appropriate key on the keyboard. At the end of every 10 trials, subjects were informed of the number of those trials on which they responded correctly. The ease or difficulty of the task was varied over trials by varying the stimulus direction of the comparison so that difficult trials contained smaller direction differences between the standard and comparison stimuli than did easy trials. This direction was determined using interleaved 2-up, 1-down and 4-up, 1-down staircases. Trials were run until there were at least 12 reversals of each staircase. A subject's approximate 71% correct and 84% correct thresholds were set to the average values over the last 10 reversals of the 2-up, 1-down and 4-up, 1-down staircases, respectively.
The audition-only training task was identical to the vision-only training task with the following exception. Instead of viewing RDK, subjects heard auditory stimuli. The standard was an auditory stimulus midway between stimuli A and B defined above, whereas the comparison was either nearer to A or nearer to B. Subjects judged whether the comparison was closer to A or B relative to the standard. Subjects were familiarized with A and B prior to performing the task.
Subjects also performed a vision–audition training task in which an auditory signal is correlated with a visual cue to motion direction. Before performing this task, we formed a relationship between visual and auditory stimuli by mapping subjects' visual thresholds onto their auditory thresholds. This was done using a log-linear function
where
d v and
d a are visual and auditory “directions,” respectively,
m is a slope parameter, and
b is an intercept parameter. The log-linear function ensured that corresponding visual and auditory stimuli were (approximately) equally salient. The vision–audition training task was identical to the vision-only training task with the following exception. Instead of only viewing RDK, subjects both viewed RDK and heard the corresponding auditory stimuli. They were instructed to focus on the visual motion-direction discrimination task but were also told that the auditory stimulus might be helpful. Half the subjects were run in the “no-switch” condition, which means that the relationship between an auditory cue and a response key was the same on this task as it was on the audition-only task. The remaining subjects were run in the “switch” condition. (In other words, for half the subjects, the stimulus direction of auditory stimulus
A was anticlockwise of vertical and the direction of
B was clockwise of vertical, whereas this relationship was reversed for the remaining subjects.) This was done so that results on the vision–audition training and test tasks could not be attributed to an association between auditory stimuli and response keys learned when subjects performed the audition-only trials.
Vision–audition test trials were conducted to evaluate whether subjects learned that the auditory signal is correlated with the visual cue to motion direction and, thus, whether it, too, is a cue to motion direction. These test trials were similar to vision–audition training trials with the following differences. First, the presentation order of the standard and comparison was randomized. Subjects were instructed to judge whether the direction of the second stimulus was anticlockwise or clockwise relative to that of the first stimulus. Second, subjects never received feedback. Third, stimuli were selected according to the method of constant stimuli rather than according to a staircase. Importantly, standard stimuli were “cue-conflict” stimuli—the direction of the RDK was vertical, but the direction of the auditory stimulus was offset from vertical by either a value δ or − δ, where δ was set to a subject's 84% correct threshold on the audition-only training trials. In contrast, the comparison stimulus was a “cue-consistent” stimulus. By comparing performances when the auditory signal in the standard had an offset of δ versus − δ, we can evaluate whether this signal influenced subjects' judgments of motion direction.
Subjects performed the four tasks during two experimental sessions. In Session 1, they performed the vision-only and audition-only training tasks. Before performing these tasks, subjects performed a small number of practice trials in which they were given feedback on every trial. They also performed the vision–audition training task twice. In Session 2, they performed the vision–audition training task and then performed the vision–audition test task twice.