One of the oldest unsolved problems in vision science is the question of how the visual system groups spatially separate elements of a display to form a coherent whole. An early exposition of this problem was provided by Ehrenfels (
1890/1988). Ehrenfels's student, Wertheimer (
1923/1938), formulated a set of “Laws of Organization,” now known as the
Gestalt laws, to describe various grouping phenomena.
The Gestalt laws amounted to observations of grouping phenomena but gave little insight into the underlying mechanisms. To achieve a better understanding of visual grouping, Field, Hayes, and Hess (
1993) introduced a new type of stimulus to investigate the grouping of elements along a contour. The target contour consisted of spatially separate elements positioned along a smooth path. The contour was embedded in a field of randomly oriented distractor elements with the same spatiotemporal properties as the contour elements. The spatial separation of the distractors was approximately matched to the separation of the contour elements, so that the contour could only be distinguished from the background by virtue of the fact that the contour elements formed a smooth path.
Most studies of contour integration have used contours in which the elements formed tangents to the path. However, a few studies have shown that it is possible to detect contours in which the elements are perpendicular to the path. These two types of contour were labeled “snakes” and “ladders,” respectively, by Bex, Simmers, and Dakin (
2001). Ladders are generally harder to detect than snakes (Bex et al.,
2001; Field et al.,
1993; Hess, Ledgeway, & Dakin,
2000; Ledgeway, Hess, & Geisler,
2005) but are much easier to detect than contours in which the elements are oriented at 45° to the path (Ledgeway et al.,
2005).
There are several different methods by which contour integration could be achieved (Watt, Dakin, & Ledgeway,
in press). These include association field mechanisms (e.g., Field et al.,
1993), Delaunay triangulation (e.g., Watt et al.,
in press), and algorithms based on spatial overlap of filter responses (e.g., Hess & Dakin,
1997). It may be that snake and ladder contours are both integrated using the same kind of mechanism. Ledgeway et al. (
2005) argued in favor of this view and sketched out an association field model in which there were strong connections between collinear elements and weak connections between parallel elements. May and Hess (
in press) implemented a model of this kind, in which snake associations were about twice as strong as ladder associations. In constructing their model, May & Hess borrowed a key feature from Pelli, Palomares, and Majaj's (
2004) model of crowding: The minimum association field size at each point in the visual field was proportional to the eccentricity. This feature allowed May and Hess's model to explain a striking difference between snakes and ladders: Straight snakes are easily detectable far into the periphery, whereas straight ladders are undetectable at quite small eccentricities.
An alternative view is that snakes and ladders are detected by completely different mechanisms. For example, snakes might be detected using an association field mechanism, whereas ladders might be detected by looking for extended regions of response in the output of a second-order channel, such as those proposed in many models of texture segregation (Graham,
1991; Graham & Sutter,
1998; Graham, Beck, & Sutter,
1992; Graham, Sutter, & Venkatesan,
1993; Lin & Wilson,
1996; Sutter, Beck, & Graham,
1989; Wilson,
1993).
To derive a realistic model of how the human visual system integrates snake and ladder contours, it is essential that we learn as much as possible about the similarities and differences between the mechanisms that detect these two types of contour. To this end, our study compared the integration speeds of snake and ladder contours. Highly curved contours are integrated more slowly than straight ones (Hess, Beaudot, & Mullen,
2001), but little else is known about the speed of contour integration.
Recently, Cass and Spehar (
2005a,
2005b) investigated the cortical propagation speed of the facilitation signals underlying the flanker facilitation effect. They argued that the facilitation signal was faster when the target and the flankers were parallel (as in the ladder contour configuration) than when they were collinear (snake configuration). This does not necessarily imply a similar difference in the integration speed of snake and ladder contours because it is unlikely that contour integration and flanker facilitation are mediated by the same mechanisms (Huang, Hess, & Dakin,
2006; Williams & Hess,
1998). However, the fact that snake and ladder configurations showed differences in the speed of one type of contextual influence suggested that it might be fruitful investigate the speed of another type of contextual influence, namely, contour integration.