Based on the categorization of all 417 colored patches in each trial, one can define a neutral point in color space. We used two different definitions of the neutral point. The neutral point was defined either as the achromatic point, which is the average of all color patches named gray, or as the convergence point of lines of equal hue. It has been shown that both points need not coincide (Ekroll, Faul, Niederee, & Richter,
2002). While the achromatic point is easy to determine, its definition is based only on a small subset of all 417 color categorizations and is vulnerable to occasional misclassifications. Further, the achromatic point is not available if the observer did not name any patch as gray. The convergence point of lines of equal hue, on the other hand, is based on almost all categorizations made and, thus, is better constrained by the measurements. To determine the convergence point, we varied the position of the point and the angles of the category boundaries. The position was varied on a discrete grid of 25 × 25 points with a spacing of 0.05 units, centered at the chromaticity of the illumination color. For each position on the grid, we first determined the optimal boundary angle between each pair of two adjacent categories and then computed the overall number of false classifications. Here, and in the following, a “false” classification means an empirical classification that was not predicted by the straight-line classification boundary. Likewise, a “correct” classification means an empirical classification that falls within the straight-line classification boundaries of that category. The point with the fewest overall false classifications was chosen as the convergence point. The optimal boundary angle between two categories was determined figuratively by rotating the angle in steps of 1° and finding the minimum number of false classifications. This operation was formalized by first determining an approximation of the category boundary from the angular mean of the average directions of the two categories. Then, the color circle was split at the angle opposite to this approximative category boundary, and a cumulative circular histogram of each color category was computed such that the value of each cumulative histogram for a particular angle gives the number of correct classifications for this category. The optimal angle is then given by the angle where the sum of the two cumulative histograms has a maximum. A color constancy index was used to quantify the degree of constancy in the different experimental conditions. This color constancy index relates the measured shift of the neutral point to the shift of the chromaticity of the illumination. The neutral point could be either the achromatic point or the convergence point of lines of equal hue. Let
in be the chromaticity coordinate of the neutral illumination ((0, 0) in the DKL space) and
ic be the chromaticity coordinate of achromatic illumination (e.g., (0.5, 0) for reddish illumination), with the corresponding neutral points measured at
an and
ac. The color constancy index
c is then defined as the ratio of the distance between the measured neutral points and the distance of the illuminations:
The degree of color constancy index is 100% if the projection coincides with the adapting background (perfect constancy) and 0% if it coincides with the white point (no effect of the background color). The same measure has been used by others (e.g., Smithson & Zaidi,
2004; Yang & Shevell,
2002).