Assuming that reduced neural activation, whether due to inhibition or habituation, was responsible for the observed contrast suppression in this study, then the results presented here have applicability to assessing a number of models of moving object perception. For example, models proposed by Fu, Shen, and Dan (
2001), Kanai, Sheth, and Shimojo (
2004; also see Sheth et al.,
2000), and Kirschfeld and Kammer (
1999; also see Kirschfeld,
2006) assumed a region of inhibition in processing regions corresponding to positions behind moving stimuli (earlier similar proposals are reviewed by Breitmeyer & Öğmen,
2006). The results of the current study are in agreement with those models.
However, Kanai et al. (
2004; also see Sheth et al.,
2000) and Kirschfeld and Kammer (
1999; also see Kirschfeld,
2006) also assumed a region of
excitation to exist in visual processing regions corresponding to positions ahead of moving stimuli. One could argue that their posited excitation is somehow specific to that moving stimulus; that is, it can only affect that moving stimulus when it reaches the region of excitation. In that case, the flashes used here would not be appropriate probes of it. However, if this position is maintained, it may be difficult to falsify. One possibility is that the excitation may only facilitate processing of other
moving stimuli—it may be possible to contrive a suitable test stimulus to test this (cf. Whitney et al.,
2003). Alternatively, one might argue that the excitation is only effective in a very small region immediately in front of the moving stimulus, and the flashes in the current work were not spatially accurate enough to probe this. However, if this were the case, the excitatory region would have to switch within a quite small distance (<≈0.5°) to an inhibitory region. Although this is possible, it seems more parsimonious to conclude that the data in this study do not support these models.
It is also of note that Glaser and Barch (
1999) have proposed and simulated a neural model of visual motion processing that exhibits what they termed “bow waves” of activity. These are entirely excitatory. Unless the speed of the moving object is small compared to the speed of propagation in the network (Barch & Glaser,
2002; in which case excitation extends ahead of the moving object), the excitation forms two wakes trailing from the outside edges of the moving object (Glaser & Barch,
1999). Again, if neural inhibition underlies the suppression reported here, existence of such wakes is not supported.
Returning to Kirschfeld and Kammer's (
1999) model, their excitatory process served to provide an explanation of the
flash-lag illusion, in which a moving stimulus displayed in alignment with a flash is perceived to spatially lead it (Krekelberg & Lappe,
2001; Nijhawan,
2002; Öğmen, Patel, Bedell, & Camuz,
2004; Schlag & Shlag-Rey,
2002; Whitney,
2002). According to Kirschfeld and Kammer, the excitatory process would allow the moving stimulus to be processed more quickly, at a given position, than the flash and, thus, would appear to spatially lead the flash. The present results suggest an alternative account. Objects displayed with less luminance are processed with a longer latency (e.g., Wilson & Anstis,
1969). More to the point, especially under conditions where the target has a shorter duration than (or is less luminous than) the mask, reaction times to targets may be increased by the presence of the mask, in a metacontrast or paracontrast paradigm (Breitmeyer & Öğmen,
2006; Kirschfeld & Kammer,
2000). Experimentation is needed to confirm my hypothesis that the flash-lag flash's processing is slowed by the moving stimulus, thus providing an alternative basis for a differential latency account of this illusion.