Current models for visuo-proprioceptive integration assume a fusion of visual and proprioceptive information in the spatial domain, with a bimodal estimate lying between the two merged, unimodal estimates (Rossetti, Desmurget, & Prablanc,
1995; Sober & Sabes,
2003; van Beers, Sittig, & Gon,
1999). In the literature on perception, however, there is evidence that different sensory modalities also interact in the temporal domain, so that a stimulus in one modality can speed up the reaction to a stimulus in a different modality when the stimuli are presented in spatial proximity (Spence, McDonald, & Driver,
2004). For instance, a key press in response to a flash of light occurs faster if a noninformative cue in a different modality is presented shortly before the visual stimulus near its location compared with far away (Kennett, Eimer, Spence, C., & Driver,
2001; Spence & Driver,
1994). This facilitation of a response to a cued stimulus has recently been shown to reflect the enhancement of visual signal by increased attention at the cued location (McDonald, Teder-Sälejärvi, & Hillyard,
2000).
During visually guided movements, visual and proprioceptive stimuli of hand location normally occur in spatial and temporal correspondence. There is general consensus that visual information reaches the CNS slower than information coming through other modalities due to the delay introduced by transduction processes in the retina's photoreceptors. Whereas the current models specify how vision and proprioception interact spatially, very little is known about their temporal interaction. We therefore tested whether proprioception speeds up the processing of visual information during a reaction time motor task. We measured whether errors in the visual hand position induced by a small rotation transformation were corrected more slowly in a condition with decreased proprioceptive accuracy. The proprioceptive deafferentation was induced in healthy subjects by 1-Hz rTMS over the somatosensory cortex (Balslev et al.,
2004).
To test for nonspecific effects of proprioceptive deafferentation on the reaction time for a motor response, the subjects also completed a control task where they corrected for a target jump, a task that does not require visual feedback from the hand (Pélisson, Prablanc, Goodale, & Jeannerod,
1986). The absence of a reaction time difference in this control task would exclude a general decrease in reaction time after proprioceptive deafferentation.
There are several ways in which interactions between visual and proprioceptive signals could influence reaction times in this task.
Firstly, the spatial fusion of visual and proprioceptive estimates into a single estimate of hand location that lies in between the visual and the proprioceptive ones (Rossetti et al.,
1995; Sober & Sabes,
2003; van Beers et al.,
1999) would predict a decrease in reaction time after proprioceptive deafferentation. This is because by reducing the proprioceptive input, the combined signal would be shifted toward the visual estimate, and thus the visual error would be more pronounced (Hypothesis 1).
Secondly, if the spatial reliability of the combined signal is considered (van Beers, Baraduc, & Wolpert,
2002), then the opposite effect may be seen. The combined estimate of hand position based on two independent noisy estimates is normally more reliable than either estimate alone. If the effect of deafferentation was to increase noise in the proprioceptive channel beyond the capacity of this mechanism for noise reduction, then the combined estimate of the hand position would become less reliable. Hence, it may take longer to perceive the deviation of the trajectory away from the target, and thus the reaction time to correct the movement would be elevated (Hypothesis 2).
Thirdly, if the two sensory modalities interact in the spatial allocation of attention (McDonald et al.,
2000), such that proprioception involuntarily draws attentional resources to the location of the visual stimulus, then proprioceptive deafferentation should slow down the response to a perturbation in visual feedback, as the visual information would be less salient without the proprioceptive cue (Hypothesis 3).
Finally, if the visuo-proprioceptive conflict rather than the visual error triggers a trajectory correction, when this conflict is reduced by proprioceptive deafferentation, the reaction time for the correction is expected to increase (Hypothesis 4).
Thus, an increase in reaction time after proprioceptive deafferentation would support Hypotheses 2, 3, or 4, whereas a decrease in reaction time would support Hypothesis 1. In addition, to find out which factors control the reaction time for a trajectory correction and thus to be able to separate between Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4, we also computed correlation coefficients between this reaction time and measures of visual reliability, visual error, and visuo-proprioceptive conflict.