Stimulus rivalry is a perceptual phenomenon resembling conventional binocular rivalry (Logothetis et al.,
1996). It is still unknown, however, to what degree these two forms of visual rivalry share common neural mechanisms. This motivated us to investigate whether the effects of TMS on stimulus rivalry are different from its effects on binocular rivalry. Therefore, we ran experiments analogous to those described above, except that in the place of conventional binocular rivalry, we presented eye-swapping stimulus rivalry. As with conventional binocular rivalry, our observers showed the characteristic peak with skewness toward long durations for stimulus rivalry (Levelt,
1965; Logothetis et al.,
1996). The distribution of dominance durations was fit well with the gamma distribution (
r2 = .94), with scale and shape parameters (4.5 and 4.4, respectively) consistent with those previously reported (Logothetis et al.,
1996).
The average dominance duration during stimulus rivalry was 2.09 s (
SD = 0.25 s), about a second shorter than that for binocular rivalry, 2.93 s (
SD = 1.2 s). This is evident when dominance duration distribution for stimulus is shown relative to that for binocular rivalry (
Figure 1C, solid blue line). Analyzing results separately for each person, three out of five observers exhibited significantly longer dominance durations during binocular rivalry (all
t > 3.47, all
df > 605, all
p < 10
-3), and one observer had a longer dominance duration during stimulus rivalry,
t(902) = 4.48,
p < 10
-5; for the remaining observer, the difference between the two conditions was nonsignificant.
We then applied TMS over a scalp position yielding central phosphenes while observers tracked stimulus rivalry alternations. Application of TMS shortened stimulus rivalry dominance durations in two out of five observers (all
t > 3.1, all
df > 546, all
p < .002) and had no effect on the remaining three observers. On average, this dominance duration shortening was relatively small (180 ms;
Figure 1C, doted blue line) and nonsignificant at a group level,
t(4) = 2.25,
p = .09 (paired
t test).
TMS applied at the scalp location eliciting
central phosphenes, however, had no effect on the timing of stimulus rivalry alternations (
p = .12;
Figure 4A, red diamonds), that is, the same TMS site that
did influence binocular rivalry. In other words, TMS at the same location and intensity, although inducing alternations in binocular rivalry, did not induce any time-locked effects on the perceptual alternations of stimulus rivalry. We also analyzed the individual results (as in
Figure 1A) and found no significant alternation probability peaks in all five observers (all
p > .34). Based on this null result, we suspect that the small and, more importantly, non-time-locked increase in alternation frequency for two observers could be related to a general elevation of arousal during the TMS experiment (e.g., Carter, Pettigrew, et al.,
2005; Carter, Presti, et al.,
2005; George,
1936).
We next attempted to perturb stimulus rivalry by replacing TMS pulses with visual flashes. A transient visual flash should send a propagating signal throughout the different stages of the visual system, and thus, we hypothesized that it should disrupt not only binocular rivalry but also stimulus rivalry. Indeed, unlike TMS, the visual flash affected stimulus rivalry (
p < 10
–5;
Figure 4, open diamonds). We also examined the relationship between the timing of flash-induced effect on stimulus rivalry and individual observers' native dominance duration for stimulus rivalry but found no correlation (
r = −.49,
p = .51), a result mirroring those for binocular rivalry (
Figure 2A). The susceptibility of stimulus rivalry to visual flash transients indicates that the null result with stimulus rivalry and TMS (
Figure 4A) is not attributable to a general insensitivity of stimulus rivalry to transient events but perhaps to stimulus rivalry not being contingent on the neural areas that we stimulated with TMS.