The human eye suffers from wave aberrations that degrade vision. The lower order aberrations, defocus and astigmatism, have been measured at least 200 years ago (Young,
1801). These lower order aberrations are corrected with spectacles, contact lenses, intraocular lenses, and refractive surgery. Higher order wave aberrations, beyond defocus and astigmatism, have been known to exist in the eye for more than 150 years (Helmholtz,
1881). Since Smirnov (
1961) used a psychophysical method to provide a description of the third and forth order aberrations, investigators have demonstrated a variety of different methods for estimating the wave aberrations of the human eye (Artal, Guirao, Berrio, & Williams,
2001; Campbell, Harrison, & Simonet,
1990; He, Marcos, Webb, & Burns,
1998; Hofer, Artal, Singer, Aragón, & Williams,
2001; Howland & Buettner,
1989; Howland & Howland,
1977; Iglesias, Berrio, & Artal,
1998; Liang, Grimm, Goelz, & Bille,
1994; Navarro & Losada,
1997; Mierdel, Krinke, Wiegand, Kaemmerer, & Seiler,
1997; Rosenblum & Christensen,
1976; Van den Brink,
1962; Walsh, Charman, & Howland,
1984). These pioneering studies greatly increased our understanding of the eye's higher order wave aberration. In recent years, the Shack–Hartmann wavefront sensor has become a popular method for wave aberration measurement with the goal of compensating these higher order aberrations to achieve diffraction-limited optics in the living eye (Fernández, Iglesias, & Artal,
2001; Hofer, Chen, Yoon, Yamauchi, & Williams,
2001; Liang & Williams,
1997) and customized vision correction with contact lens, intraocular lenses, and refractive surgery. However, the subjective image quality of the human eye depends not only on the optical blur caused by the wave aberrations but also on neural factors and the experience of the observer (George & Rosenfield,
2004; Mon-Williams, Tresilian, Strang, Kochhar, & Wann,
1998; Pesudovs & Brennan,
1993; Rosenfield & Hong,
2001; Rosenfield, Hong, & George,
2004; Watt,
1987; Webster,
2005; Webster, Georgeson, & Webster,
2002). This fact is well known among clinicians who often use a two step procedure to achieve a full correction of astigmatism, allowing time between the two steps for the patient's nervous system to adjust to a partial correction. There is adaptation not only to defocus and astigmatism, but also to the particular pattern of higher order aberrations (Artal et al.,
2004). The subjective blur produced when viewing a scene through one's own wave aberration was less than that when the wave aberration was rotated. In the present paper, we investigate whether this neural compensation modifies the amount of aberration correction that produces the best subjective image quality.