Ascertaining the extent to which visual perception is influenced by a perceiver's cultural background is important not only because of basic brain research with humans, but also because of the implications for animal research and for government policies (Cyranoski,
2006; Editorial,
2006). Our data do not support the hypothesis advocated in Kitayama et al. (
2003) that basic visual perception is influenced by the perceiver's cultural background. Instead, the data could be explained by a simpler notion that a visible frame of reference helps encode more precisely a line's length relative to the frame than the same line's length independent of the frame (Rock & Ebenholtz,
1959).
This simpler explanation is also consistent with our results that when the size information of the frame was degraded—that is, when the frame was presented as white paper cutout against light background—the advantage of the relative task over the absolute task diminished. In no experiments in our study, however, was the absolute task more accurate than the relative task. A particularly interesting case is when the first and second frames were identical, where the solutions were also identical in both tasks. Because the stimuli were identical, any differential results could only be due to experimental instructions. Not surprisingly, because the frame size in the absolute task was irrelevant and presumably ignored by the participants, the absolute task was more error prone than the relative task.
Our results are also consistent with recent results that contradicted, at least partially, earlier findings by Nisbett and colleagues (Chang, Rotello, Li, & Rayner,
2008; Chen & Jiang,
2007; Rayner, Li, Williams, Cave, & Well,
2007). Although we cannot explain why the results are different between our experiments and those in Kitayama et al. (
2003), it is worthwhile to list our methodological differences.
-
The Asian participants in Kitayama et al. (
2003) were Japanese, whereas ours were Chinese. However, the discrepancy between the two studies is not between the Japanese and Chinese. The discrepancy is between the American participants in the two studies.
-
In Kitayama et al. (
2003), the sequential order of the five trials in the absolute task and that of the five trials in the relative task was fixed across participants (while the task order was counterbalanced). We used both fixed and randomized sequences with counterbalancing. However, we do not expect this to be critical.
-
In Kitayama et al. (
2003), a trial's first stimulus, a line in a square, was shown to a participant at one corner of the laboratory, whereas the second stimulus, the second square, was shown at a different corner. The purpose was to prevent the use of iconic memory. We did not move a participant from one place to another in the laboratory. By ensuring that no two stimuli were shown simultaneously, and given the short duration of iconic memory (Sperling,
1960), we believe that iconic memory was unlikely to play any role in the tasks. We also recruited and tested participants outdoors but could not find any difference between the indoor and outdoor results.
In conclusion, under no conditions could we find that reproduction of a line's absolute length was better than reproduction of length relative to the square enclosing it. This result is sensible in light of available information: a line estimated relative to a visible frame of reference is expected to be more accurate than encoded into memory without reference to any frame of reference.