When different images project to corresponding regions on the retinas of the two eyes, each image can intermittently disappear such that only one of the two is perceived at a time—a phenomenon known as binocular rivalry (Helmholtz,
1962; Levelt,
1968). It may be tempting to dismiss binocular rivalry as being curious but irrelevant in the context of our daily lives. How often, after all, do we usually encounter completely different images at corresponding points in the two eyes?
The answer is frequently (see Shimojo & Nakayama,
1990,
1994), but this does not normally induce binocular rivalry. For instance, at times, humans try to maximize the probability of seeing without being seen. We do so by exposing the minimal bodily extent (one eye) necessary to see—we peek around corners. When both eyes are open, this creates a situation wherein one eye is selectively occluded and, as a consequence, completely different images project to either eye. However, as interested readers can readily demonstrate for themselves (by placing a fingertip a few centimeters in front of one eye while fixating this text), peeking does not typically induce binocular rivalry. Instead, perception tends to be persistently dominated by that which is closest in depth to the point of fixation (hence, one can still read this text). We are usually only faintly aware of the retinal image projecting from the more proximate object or surface that we are peeking around.
In virtually all viewing conditions, the eyes accommodate at the point of fixation, rendering images of objects and surfaces displaced in depth from fixation relatively blurred. Given the almost complete perceptual suppression of more proximate objects and surfaces when peeking, it is perhaps unsurprising that blur magnitude is negatively correlated with the probability of perceptual dominance during binocular rivalry (Fahle,
1982,
1983; Levelt,
1968) and in possibly related clinical situations (Schor, Landsman, & Erickson,
1987; Shors, Wright, & Greene,
1992).
Because of the manner in which binocular rivalry experiments are usually conducted, with each of the conflicting images presented selectively to either eye, it is unclear if blur biases perception in favor of a particular stimulus (Dorrenhaus,
1975; Logothetis, Leopold, & Sheinberg,
1996) or in favor of information from a specific eye (Blake, Westendorf, & Overton,
1980; Polonsky, Blake, Braun, & Heeger,
2000; Tong & Engel,
2001). We believe that there is a third possibility—that, as a selection criterion, blur can be independent of both eye of origin and stimulus type. Our thoughts are as follows: The perceptual suppression of the more blurred of two conflicting images (Fahle,
1982,
1983; Levelt,
1968), as experienced while peeking, is consistent with the existence of an evolutionary adaptation that facilitates visibility near the point of fixation. The point of fixation has two interesting characteristics—it is a property that is tied neither to a particular stimulus nor to a specific eye.
Take, for example, the situation where one gazes at a distant object through the leaves of a tree. Because of movement (of either the head or the leaves), the status of the distant object will often fluctuate between being unobscured, thus projecting to corresponding points in both eyes and being obscured from one eye or the other (for a related example, see Ono, Lillakas, Grove, & Suzuki,
2003). Thus, the fixation point can alternate rapidly between projecting to either eye. We can also change our point of fixation between different objects. To promote visibility near the point of fixation, over that of more proximate objects and surfaces, it would therefore be important to discount information concerning both eye of origin and stimulus type and rely instead on a more consistent cue—relative image blur.
The dominant views concerning binocular rivalry suggest that suppression is related to selections of a particular stimulus (Dorrenhaus,
1975; Logothetis et al.,
1996) or to information from a specific eye (Blake et al.,
1980; Polonsky et al.,
2000; Tong & Engel,
2001). The third possibility—that suppression might be independently related to distance in depth from fixation—is a novel suggestion that has not yet been assessed. We address this possibility using manipulations of image blur, to cue relative distance in depth from fixation, in combination with several image-swapping procedures. These swapping procedures allow us to break the links between (a) relative image blur and eye of origin and (b) relative image blur and stimulus type. By breaking these links, we can establish if image blur can determine suppression selectivity during binocular rivalry independent of information concerning both eye of origin and stimulus type.